全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

An Exploration of Responses to Drug Conditioned Stimuli during Treatment for Substance Dependence

DOI: 10.1155/2013/394064

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Although it is well established that drug conditioned stimuli produce a variety of conditioned responses, it is not known whether such stimuli can also reinforce an arbitrary operant response and thus serve as conditioned reinforcers. Volunteers ( ) recruited from a residential treatment center for substance dependence were tested on a task in which presses on computer keys activated images of drugs/drug paraphernalia on a progressive ratio schedule of reinforcement. They also completed a personalized craving questionnaire and a personalized Implicit Association Test. A significant bias in responding was found for images of preferred drugs/route of drug administration. Craving, however, was low and the images generated negative evaluative reactions. Two additional studies were performed to ascertain the generalizability of the effects to a different population of drug-using individuals (i.e., students who drink) and to incentive stimuli of a different nature (i.e., sexual). The additional studies partially replicated and extended the central findings of the main study. Therefore, although these data should be considered preliminary in light of small group sizes, it is concluded that cue specificity and availability of the unconditioned stimuli (drugs and sex) plays a role in modulating responding maintained by conditioned reinforcers. 1. Introduction Drug conditioned stimuli, which can be discrete (i.e., a syringe) and/or environmental (i.e., a room), acquire the ability to activate drug-oriented behaviors because they are repeatedly perceived in conjunction with the unconditioned effects of drugs of abuse [1–3]. Hence, through Pavlovian conditioning, drug conditioned stimuli become wanted [4] and preferred [5], grab attention [6–8], and produce a variety of physiological and psychological responses [9–15]. The current study had two primary objectives. The first was to establish whether drug conditioned stimuli (i.e., images of drugs and drug paraphernalia) can serve as conditioned reinforcers. Conditioned reinforcing stimuli, unlike primary reinforcing stimuli, strengthen behavioral responses in virtue of their learned value. Therefore, the objective of this study was to determine whether the occurrence of stimuli associated with the effects of drugs can maintain an arbitrary operant response (i.e., pressing a computer key) in the absence of drugs [16]. This is of interest because it is possible that the assessment of the reinforcing value of drug conditioned stimuli could complement other measures of “cue reactivity” such as self-reported craving [17,

References

[1]  J. Stewart, “Conditioned and unconditioned drug effects in relapse to opiate and stimulant drug self-administration,” Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, vol. 7, no. 4–6, pp. 591–597, 1983.
[2]  J. Stewart, H. de Wit, and R. Eikelboom, “Role of unconditioned and conditioned drug effects in the self-administration of opiates and stimulants,” Psychological Review, vol. 91, no. 2, pp. 251–268, 1984.
[3]  A. R. Childress, R. Ehrman, D. J. Rohsenow, et al., “Classically conditioned factors in drug dependence. Substance abuse: a comprehensive textbook,” Baltimore, pp. 56–69, 1992.
[4]  T. E. Robinson and K. C. Berridge, “The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction,” Brain Research Reviews, vol. 18, no. 3, pp. 247–291, 1993.
[5]  S. J. Moeller, T. Maloney, M. A. Parvaz et al., “Enhanced choice for viewing cocaine pictures in cocaine addiction,” Biological Psychiatry, vol. 66, no. 2, pp. 169–176, 2009.
[6]  I. H. A. Franken, “Drug craving and addiction: integrating psychological and neuropsychopharmacological approaches,” Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, vol. 27, no. 4, pp. 563–579, 2003.
[7]  M. Field and W. M. Cox, “Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: a review of its development, causes, and consequences,” Drug and Alcohol Dependence, vol. 97, no. 1-2, pp. 1–20, 2008.
[8]  D. I. Lubman, L. A. Peters, K. Mogg, B. P. Bradley, and J. F. W. Deakin, “Attentional bias for drug cues in opiate dependence,” Psychological Medicine, vol. 30, no. 1, pp. 169–175, 2000.
[9]  C. P. O'Brien, T. Testa, T. J. O'Brien, and R. Greenstein, “Conditioning in human opiate addicts,” Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science, vol. 11, no. 4, pp. 195–202, 1976.
[10]  C. P. O'Brien, A. R. Childress, A. T. McLellan, R. Ehrman, and J. W. Ternes, “Types of conditioning found in drug-dependent humans,” NIDA Research Monograph Series, no. 84, pp. 44–61, 1988.
[11]  A. R. Childress, P. D. Mozley, W. McElgin, J. Fitzgerald, M. Reivich, and C. P. O'Brien, “Limbic activation during cue-induced cocaine craving,” American Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 156, no. 1, pp. 11–18, 1999.
[12]  N. D. Volkow, G.-J. Wang, F. Telang et al., “Cocaine cues and dopamine in dorsal striatum: mechanism of craving in cocaine addiction,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 26, no. 24, pp. 6583–6588, 2006.
[13]  B. L. Carter and S. T. Tiffany, “Meta-analysis of cue-reactivity in addiction research,” Addiction, vol. 94, no. 3, pp. 327–340, 1999.
[14]  M. D. Skinner and H.-J. Aubin, “Craving's place in addiction theory: contributions of the major models,” Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, vol. 34, no. 4, pp. 606–623, 2010.
[15]  R. Sinha, T. Fuse, L.-R. Aubin, and S. S. O'Malley, “Psychological stress, drug-related cues and cocaine craving,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 152, no. 2, pp. 140–148, 2000.
[16]  W. M. Davis and S. G. Smith, “Role of conditioned reinforcers in the initiation, maintenance and extinction of drug seeking behavior,” Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science, vol. 11, no. 4, pp. 222–236, 1976.
[17]  M. A. Sayette, S. Shiffman, S. T. Tiffany, R. S. Niaura, C. S. Martin, and W. G. Schadel, “The measurement of drug craving,” Addiction, vol. 95, supplement 2, pp. S189–S210, 2000.
[18]  D. C. Drummond, R. Z. Litten, C. Lowman, and W. A. Hunt, “Craving research: future directions,” Addiction, vol. 95, supplement 2, pp. S247–S255, 2000.
[19]  D. C. Drummond, “What does cue-reactivity have to offer clinical research?” Addiction, vol. 95, supplement 2, pp. S129–S144, 2000.
[20]  S. T. Tiffany and J. M. Wray, “The clinical significance of drug craving,” Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol. 1248, no. 1, pp. 1–17, 2012.
[21]  G. Leon, “Therapeutic communities,” in Textbook of Substance Abuse Treatment, M. K. Galanter and D. De Herbert, Eds., American Psychiatric Press, Washington, DC, USA, 2nd edition, 1999.
[22]  R. W. Wiers, N. Van Woerden, F. T. Y. Smulders, and P. J. de Jong, “Implicit and explicit alcohol-related cognitions in heavy and light drinkers,” Journal of Abnormal Psychology, vol. 111, no. 4, pp. 648–658, 2002.
[23]  S. T. Tiffany, “A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: role of automatic and nonautomatic processes,” Psychological Review, vol. 97, no. 2, pp. 147–168, 1990.
[24]  T. E. Robinson and K. C. Berridge, “Addiction,” Annual Review of Psychology, vol. 13, pp. 155–162, 2003.
[25]  J. G. Pfaus, T. E. Kippin, and S. Centeno, “Conditioning and sexual behavior: a review,” Hormones and Behavior, vol. 40, no. 2, pp. 291–321, 2001.
[26]  F. D. Garcia and F. Thibaut, “Sexual addictions,” American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, vol. 36, no. 5, pp. 254–260, 2010.
[27]  A. Love, D. James, and P. Willner, “A comparison of two alcohol craving questionnaires,” Addiction, vol. 93, no. 7, pp. 1091–1102, 1998.
[28]  M. J. Bohn, D. D. Krahn, and B. A. Staehler, “Development and initial validation of a measure of drinking urges in abstinent alcoholics,” Alcoholism, vol. 19, no. 3, pp. 600–606, 1995.
[29]  B. D. Sussner, D. A. Smelson, S. Rodrigues, A. Kline, M. Losonczy, and D. Ziedonis, “The validity and reliability of a brief measure of cocaine craving,” Drug and Alcohol Dependence, vol. 83, no. 3, pp. 233–237, 2006.
[30]  S. T. Tiffany, E. Singleton, C. A. Haertzen, and J. E. Henningfield, “The development of a cocaine craving questionnaire,” Drug and Alcohol Dependence, vol. 34, no. 1, pp. 19–28, 1993.
[31]  I. H. A. Franken, V. M. Hendriks, and W. van den Brink, “Initial validation of two opiate craving questionnaires: the Obsessive Compulsive Drug Use Scale and the Desires for Drug Questionnaire,” Addictive Behaviors, vol. 27, no. 5, pp. 675–685, 2002.
[32]  D. James, G. Davies, and P. Willner, “The development and initial validation of a questionnaire to measure craving for amphetamine,” Addiction, vol. 99, no. 9, pp. 1181–1188, 2004.
[33]  A. J. Heinz, D. H. Epstein, J. R. Schroeder, E. G. Singleton, S. J. Heishman, and K. L. Preston, “Heroin and cocaine craving and use during treatment: measurement validation and potential relationships,” Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, vol. 31, no. 4, pp. 355–364, 2006.
[34]  W. Ooteman, M. W. J. Koeter, R. Vserheul, G. M. Schippers, and W. Van Den Brink, “Measuring craving: an attempt to connect subjective craving with cue reactivity,” Alcoholism, vol. 30, no. 1, pp. 57–69, 2006.
[35]  S. J. Heishman, E. G. Singleton, and A. Liguori, “Marijuana Craving Questionnaire: development and initial validation of a self-report instrument,” Addiction, vol. 96, no. 7, pp. 1023–1034, 2001.
[36]  L. S. Cox, S. T. Tiffany, and A. G. Christen, “Evaluation of the brief questionnaire of smoking urges (QSU-brief) in laboratory and clinical settings,” Nicotine and Tobacco Research, vol. 3, no. 1, pp. 7–16, 2001.
[37]  S. J. Heishman, E. G. Singleton, and E. T. Moolchan, “Tobacco Craving Questionnaire: reliability and validity of a new multifactorial instrument,” Nicotine and Tobacco Research, vol. 5, no. 5, pp. 645–654, 2003.
[38]  P. Di Ciano and B. J. Everitt, “Differential control over drug-seeking behavior by drug-associated conditioned reinforcers and discriminative stimuli predictive of drug availability,” Behavioral Neuroscience, vol. 117, no. 5, pp. 952–960, 2003.
[39]  P. Di Ciano and B. J. Everitt, “Conditioned reinforcing properties of stimuli paired with self-administered cocaine, heroin or sucrose: implications for the persistence of addictive behaviour,” Neuropharmacology, vol. 47, supplement 1, pp. 202–213, 2004.
[40]  B. Goddard and F. Leri, “Reinstatement of conditioned reinforcing properties of cocaine-conditioned stimuli,” Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, vol. 83, no. 4, pp. 540–546, 2006.
[41]  S. L. Grella, A. Levy, A. Campbell et al., “Oxycodone dose-dependently imparts conditioned reinforcing properties to discrete sensory stimuli in rats,” Pharmacological Research, vol. 64, no. 4, pp. 364–370, 2011.
[42]  E. L. Gardner, “What we have learned about addiction from animal models of drug self-administration,” American Journal on Addictions, vol. 9, no. 4, pp. 285–313, 2000.
[43]  S. C. Sigmon, J. W. Tidey, G. J. Badger, and S. T. Higgins, “Acute effects of D-amphetamine on progressive-ratio performance maintained by cigarette smoking and money,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 167, no. 4, pp. 393–402, 2003.
[44]  W. W. Stoops, A. R. Vansickel, J. A. Lile, and C. R. Rush, “Acute d-amphetamine pretreatment does not alter stimulant self-administration in humans,” Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, vol. 87, no. 1, pp. 20–29, 2007.
[45]  D. C. S. Roberts and S. A. L. Bennett, “Heroin self-administration in rats under a progressive ratio schedule of reinforcement,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 111, no. 2, pp. 215–218, 1993.
[46]  M. A. Olson and R. H. Fazio, “Reducing the influence of extrapersonal associations on the implicit association test: personalizing the IAT,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 86, no. 5, pp. 653–667, 2004.
[47]  J. E. S. Swanson, L. A. Rudman, and A. G. Greenwald, “Using the implicit association test to investigate attitude-behaviour consistency for stigmatised behaviour,” Cognition and Emotion, vol. 15, no. 2, pp. 207–230, 2001.
[48]  R. W. Wiers, A. W. Stacy, S. L. Ames et al., “Implicit and explicit alcohol-related cognitions,” Alcoholism, vol. 26, no. 1, pp. 129–137, 2002.
[49]  M. Field, K. Mogg, and B. P. Bradley, “Cognitive bias and drug craving in recreational cannabis users,” Drug and Alcohol Dependence, vol. 74, no. 1, pp. 105–111, 2004.
[50]  K. Houben and R. W. Wiers, “Are drinkers implicitly positive about drinking alcohol? Personalizing the alcohol-IAT to reduce negative extrapersonal contamination,” Alcohol and Alcoholism, vol. 42, no. 4, pp. 301–307, 2007.
[51]  J. De Houwer, R. Custers, and A. De Clercq, “Do smokers have a negative implicit attitude toward smoking?” Cognition and Emotion, vol. 20, no. 8, pp. 1274–1284, 2006.
[52]  H. E. Ross, D. R. Gavin, and H. A. Skinner, “Diagnostic validity of the MAST and the alcohol dependence scale in the assessment of DSM-III alcohol disorders,” Journal of Studies on Alcohol, vol. 51, no. 6, pp. 506–513, 1990.
[53]  L. C. Sobell, J. Brown, G. I. Leo, and M. B. Sobell, “The reliability of the Alcohol Timeline Followback when administered by telephone and by computer,” Drug and Alcohol Dependence, vol. 42, no. 1, pp. 49–54, 1996.
[54]  A. G. Greenwald, B. A. Nosek, and M. R. Banaji, “Understanding and using the implicit association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 85, no. 2, pp. 197–216, 2003.
[55]  I. H. Franken, “Drug craving and addiction: integrating psychological and neuropsychopharmacological approaches,” Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, vol. 27, pp. 563–579, 2003.
[56]  S. J. Moeller, N. Beebe-Wang, P. A. Woicik, A. B. Konova, T. Maloney, and R. Z. Goldstein, “Choice to view cocaine images predicts concurrent and prospective drug use in cocaine addiction,” Drug and Alcohol Dependence, vol. 130, no. 1–3, pp. 178–185, 2012.
[57]  C. P. O'Brien, A. R. Childress, R. Ehrman, and S. J. Robbins, “Conditioning factors in drug abuse: can they explain compulsion?” Journal of Psychopharmacology, vol. 12, no. 1, pp. 15–22, 1998.
[58]  J. M. Townshend and T. Duka, “Attentional bias associated with alcohol cues: differences between heavy and occasional social drinkers,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 157, no. 1, pp. 67–74, 2001.
[59]  K. M. Stormark, N. P. Field, K. Hugdahl, and M. Horowitz, “Selective processing of visual alcohol cues in abstinent alcoholics: an approach-avoidance conflict?” Addictive Behaviors, vol. 22, no. 4, pp. 509–519, 1997.
[60]  C. A. Conklin, K. A. Perkins, N. Robin, F. J. McClernon, and R. P. Salkeld, “Bringing the real world into the laboratory: personal smoking and nonsmoking environments,” Drug and Alcohol Dependence, vol. 111, no. 1-2, pp. 58–63, 2010.
[61]  R. E. Meyer and S. M. Mirin, The Heroin Stimulus, Implications for a Theory of Addiction, Plenum, New York, NY, USA, 1979.
[62]  S. R. Bailey, K. C. Goedeker, and S. T. Tiffany, “The impact of cigarette deprivation and cigarette availability on cue-reactivity in smokers,” Addiction, vol. 105, no. 2, pp. 364–372, 2010.
[63]  D. McBride, S. P. Barrett, J. T. Kelly, A. Aw, and A. Dagher, “Effects of expectancy and abstinence on the neural response to smoking cues in cigarette smokers: an fMRI study,” Neuropsychopharmacology, vol. 31, no. 12, pp. 2728–2738, 2006.
[64]  J. P. De Los Cobos, N. Si?ol, J. Trujols, E. Ba?uls, F. Batlle, and A. Tejero, “Drug-dependent inpatients reporting continuous absence of spontaneous drug craving for the main substance throughout detoxification treatment,” Drug and Alcohol Review, vol. 30, no. 4, pp. 403–410, 2011.
[65]  J. De Houwer, “A structural and process analysis of the Implicit Association Test,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 37, no. 6, pp. 443–451, 2001.
[66]  K. Houben and R. W. Wiers, “Personalizing the alcohol-IAT with individualized stimuli: relationship with drinking behavior and drinking-related problems,” Addictive Behaviors, vol. 32, no. 12, pp. 2852–2864, 2007.
[67]  L. J. Beckman, “Women alcoholics. A review of social and psychological studies,” Journal of Studies on Alcohol, vol. 36, no. 7, pp. 797–824, 1975.
[68]  R. S. Brienza and M. D. Stein, “Alcohol use disorders in primary care: do gender-specific differences exist?” Journal of General Internal Medicine, vol. 17, no. 5, pp. 387–397, 2002.
[69]  S. Nolen-Hoeksema, “Gender differences in risk factors and consequences for alcohol use and problems,” Clinical Psychology Review, vol. 24, no. 8, pp. 981–1010, 2004.
[70]  G. De Leon, J. Hawke, N. Jainchill, and G. Melnick, “Therapeutic communities: enhancing retention in treatment using “Senior Professor” staff,” Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, vol. 19, no. 4, pp. 375–382, 2000.
[71]  Y.-X. Xue, Y.-X. Luo, P. Wu et al., “A memory retrieval-extinction procedure to prevent drug craving and relapse,” Science, vol. 336, no. 6078, pp. 241–245, 2012.

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133