全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Higher Cortisol Predicts Less Improvement in Verbal Memory Performance after Cardiac Rehabilitation in Patients with Coronary Artery Disease

DOI: 10.1155/2013/340342

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Objective. While physical activity can improve verbal memory performance in subjects with coronary artery disease (CAD), there is large variability in response. Elevated cortisol production has been suggested to negatively affect verbal memory performance, yet cortisol concentrations have not been assessed as a predictor of response to exercise intervention in those with CAD. Methods. CAD patients participating in a one-year cardiac rehabilitation program were recruited. Memory was assessed with the California Verbal Learning Test second edition at baseline and one year. Cortisol was measured from a 20?mg, 3.0?cm hair sample collected at baseline. Results. In patients with CAD ( , mean?±?SD age?=? , 86% male), higher cortisol (hair cortisol concentrations?≥?153.2?ng/g) significantly predicted less memory improvement ( , ) when controlling for age ( , ), gender ( , ), maximal oxygen uptake ( , ), and body mass index ( , ). Conclusion. Prolonged hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis activation may interfere with exercise-related improvements in memory in CAD. 1. Introduction Coronary artery disease (CAD) is associated with an increased rate of decline in verbal memory [1]. For those with CAD, cognitive performance represents a significant determinant of long-term quality of life [2] as exemplified by associations with poor outcomes including unemployment, mortality, and failure to comply with risk factor management [2–4]. However, the mechanisms underlying cognitive changes in CAD are incompletely understood. Epidemiological studies show that cardiopulmonary fitness is associated with better cognitive performance in older subjects [5], particularly in those with CAD [6]. Exercise interventions, such as cardiac rehabilitation (CR), can improve cognitive performance, although interindividual responses to exercise training can vary [7]. CAD and related cardiovascular risk factors have also been associated with higher cortisol concentrations [8]. While prolonged elevations in cortisol have been associated with memory impairment [9], dysregulation of the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis has not been probed as a predictor of cognitive response to exercise in this population. The objective of the present study was to assess the relationship between baseline hair cortisol concentrations and change in verbal memory performance in subjects with CAD undertaking CR. Hair cortisol has been suggested to reflect longer term cortisol secretion [10, 11] rather than state concentrations, suggesting a potential clinically useful marker. We hypothesized that cortisol

References

[1]  D. J. Vinkers, M. L. Stek, R. C. van der Mast et al., “Generalized atherosclerosis, cognitive decline, and depressive symptoms in old age,” Neurology, vol. 65, no. 1, pp. 107–112, 2005.
[2]  A. Kiessling and P. Henriksson, “Perceived cognitive function in coronary artery disease—an unrecognised predictor of unemployment,” Quality of Life Research, vol. 14, no. 6, pp. 1481–1488, 2005.
[3]  C. R. Gale, C. N. Martyn, and C. Cooper, “Cognitive impairment and mortality in a cohort of elderly people,” British Medical Journal, vol. 312, no. 7031, pp. 607–611, 1996.
[4]  W. Swardfager, N. Herrmann, S. Marzolini et al., “Verbal memory performance and completion of cardiac rehabilitation in patients with coronary artery disease,” Psychosomatic Medicine, vol. 73, no. 7, pp. 580–587, 2011.
[5]  D. E. Barnes, K. Yaffe, W. A. Satariano, and I. B. Tager, “A longitudinal study of cardiorespiratory fitness and cognitive function in healthy older adults,” Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, vol. 51, no. 4, pp. 459–465, 2003.
[6]  W. Swardfager, N. Herrmann, S. Marzolini et al., “Cardiopulmonary fitness is associated with cognitive performance in patients with coronary artery disease,” Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, vol. 58, no. 8, pp. 1519–1525, 2010.
[7]  K. M. Stanek, J. Gunstad, M. B. Spitznagel et al., “Improvements in cognitive function following cardiac rehabilitation for older adults with cardiovascular disease,” International Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 121, no. 2, pp. 86–93, 2011.
[8]  D. Pereg, R. Gow, M. Mosseri et al., “Hair cortisol and the risk for acute myocardial infarction in adult men,” Stress, vol. 14, no. 1, pp. 73–81, 2011.
[9]  S. J. Lupien, M. de Leon, S. de Santi et al., “Cortisol levels during human aging predict hippocampal atrophy and memory deficits,” Nature Neuroscience, vol. 1, no. 1, pp. 69–73, 1998.
[10]  S. Thomson, G. Koren, L. A. Fraser, M. Rieder, T. C. Friedman, and S. H. M. van Uum, “Hair analysis provides a historical record of cortisol levels in Cushing's syndrome,” Experimental and Clinical Endocrinology and Diabetes, vol. 118, no. 2, pp. 133–138, 2010.
[11]  B. Sauvé, G. Koren, G. Walsh, S. Tokmakejian, and S. H. M. van Uum, “Measurement of cortisol in human hair as a biomarker of systemic exposure,” Clinical and Investigative Medicine, vol. 30, no. 5, pp. E183–E191, 2007.
[12]  R. V. Milani, C. J. Lavie, M. R. Mehra, and H. O. Ventura, “Understanding the basics of cardiopulmonary exercise testing,” Mayo Clinic Proceedings, vol. 81, no. 12, pp. 1603–1611, 2006.
[13]  K. Yaffe, T. Blackwell, A. M. Kanaya, N. Davidowitz, E. Barrett-Connor, and K. Krueger, “Diabetes, impaired fasting glucose, and development of cognitive impairment in older women,” Neurology, vol. 63, no. 4, pp. 658–663, 2004.
[14]  B. W. J. H. Penninx, A. T. F. Beekman, A. Honig et al., “Depression and cardiac mortality: results from a community-based longitudinal study,” Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 58, no. 3, pp. 221–227, 2001.
[15]  V. Elderkin-Thompson, J. Mintz, E. Haroon, H. Lavretsky, and A. Kumar, “Executive dysfunction and memory in older patients with major and minor depression,” Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, vol. 21, no. 7, pp. 669–676, 2006.
[16]  S. Cohen, T. Kamarck, and R. Mermelstein, “A global measure of perceived stress,” Journal of Health and Social Behavior, vol. 24, no. 4, pp. 385–396, 1983.
[17]  R. Gow, S. Thomson, M. Rieder, S. van Uum, and G. Koren, “An assessment of cortisol analysis in hair and its clinical applications,” Forensic Science International, vol. 196, no. 1–3, pp. 32–37, 2010.
[18]  J. Yamada, B. Stevens, N. de Silva et al., “Hair cortisol as a potential biologic marker of chronic stress in hospitalized neonates,” Neonatology, vol. 92, no. 1, pp. 42–49, 2007.
[19]  S. H. M. van Uum, B. Sauvé, L. A. Fraser, P. Morley-Forster, T. L. Paul, and G. Koren, “Elevated content of cortisol in hair of patients with severe chronic pain: a novel biomarker for stress,” Stress, vol. 11, no. 6, pp. 483–488, 2008.
[20]  T. Stalder, C. Kirschbaum, K. Heinze et al., “Use of hair cortisol analysis to detect hypercortisolism during active drinking phases in alcohol-dependent individuals,” Biological Psychology, vol. 85, no. 3, pp. 357–360, 2010.
[21]  L. Dettenborn, A. Tietze, F. Bruckner, and C. Kirschbaum, “Higher cortisol content in hair among long-term unemployed individuals compared to controls,” Psychoneuroendocrinology, vol. 35, no. 9, pp. 1404–1409, 2010.
[22]  S. Kalra, A. Einarson, T. Karaskov, S. van Uum, and G. Koren, “The relationship between stress and hair cortisol in healthy pregnant women,” Clinical and Investigative Medicine, vol. 30, no. 2, pp. E103–E107, 2007.
[23]  “Society of hair testing,” Forensic Science International, vol. 84, pp. 3–6, 1997.
[24]  R. Wennig, “Potential problems with the interpretation of hair analysis results,” Forensic Science International, vol. 107, no. 1–3, pp. 5–12, 2000.
[25]  Y. Dowlati, N. Herrmann, W. Swardfager et al., “Relationship between hair cortisol concentrations and depressive symptoms in patients with coronary artery disease,” Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, vol. 6, pp. 393–400, 2010.
[26]  L. Gerritsen, M. I. Geerlings, A. T. Beekman, D. J. Deeg, B. W. Penninx, and H. C. Comijs, “Early and late life events and salivary cortisol in older persons,” Psychological Medicine, vol. 40, no. 9, pp. 1569–1578, 2009.
[27]  B. M. Kudielka, J. Hellhammer, D. H. Hellhammer et al., “Sex differences in endocrine and psychological responses to psychosocial stress in healthy elderly subjects and the impact of a 2-week dehydroepiandrosterone treatment,” Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, vol. 83, no. 5, pp. 1756–1761, 1998.
[28]  D. Laurin, R. Verreault, J. Lindsay, K. MacPherson, and K. Rockwood, “Physical activity and risk of cognitive impairment and dementia in elderly persons,” Archives of Neurology, vol. 58, no. 3, pp. 498–504, 2001.
[29]  E. F. van Rossum and S. W. Lamberts, “Polymorphisms in the glucocorticoid receptor gene and their associations with metabolic parameters and body composition,” Recent Progress in Hormone Research, vol. 59, pp. 333–357, 2004.
[30]  W. Swardfager, N. Herrmann, Y. Dowlati, P. Oh, A. Kiss, and K. L. Lanct?t, “Relationship between cardiopulmonary fitness and depressive symptoms in cardiac rehabilitation patients with coronary artery disease,” Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, vol. 40, no. 3, pp. 213–218, 2008.
[31]  D. Mungas, W. J. Jagust, B. R. Reed et al., “MRI predictors of cognition in subcortical ischemic vascular disease and Alzheimer's disease,” Neurology, vol. 57, no. 12, pp. 2229–2235, 2001.
[32]  R. M. Sapolsky, H. Uno, C. S. Rebert, and C. E. Finch, “Hippocampal damage associated with prolonged glucocorticoid exposure in primates,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 10, no. 9, pp. 2897–2902, 1990.
[33]  M. N. Starkman, S. S. Gebarski, S. Berent, and D. E. Schteingart, “Hippocampal formation volume, memory dysfunction, and cortisol levels in patients with Cushing's syndrome,” Biological Psychiatry, vol. 32, no. 9, pp. 756–765, 1992.
[34]  F. Magri, F. Terenzi, T. Ricciardi et al., “Association between changes in adrenal secretion and cerebral morphometric correlates in normal aging and senile dementia,” Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, vol. 11, no. 2, pp. 90–99, 2000.
[35]  B. S. McEwen, “Plasticity of the hippocampus: adaptation to chronic stress and allostatic load,” Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol. 933, pp. 265–277, 2001.
[36]  M. J. M. Schaaf, E. R. de Kloet, and E. Vreugdenhil, “Corticosterone effects on bdnf expression in the hippocampus implications for memory formation,” Stress, vol. 3, no. 3, pp. 201–208, 2000.
[37]  M. F. Newman, J. L. Kirchner, B. Phillips-Bute et al., “Longitudinal assessment of neurocognitive function after coronary-artery bypass surgery,” The New England Journal of Medicine, vol. 344, no. 6, pp. 395–402, 2001.
[38]  Y. Q. Yin, A. L. Luo, X. Y. Guo, L. H. Li, and Y. G. Huang, “Postoperative neuropsychological change and its underlying mechanism in patients undergoing coronary artery bypass grafting,” Chinese Medical Journal, vol. 120, no. 22, pp. 1951–1957, 2007.
[39]  A. K. Roth-Isigkeit and P. Schmucker, “Postoperative dissociation of blood levels of cortisol and adrenocorticotropin after coronary artery bypass grafting surgery,” Steroids, vol. 62, no. 11, pp. 695–699, 1997.
[40]  C. Bartels, M. Wegrzyn, A. Wiedl, V. Ackermann, and H. Ehrenreich, “Practice effects in healthy adults: a longitudinal study on frequent repetitive cognitive testing,” BMC Neuroscience, vol. 11, article 118, 2010.

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133