全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Parasympathetic Response Profiles Related to Social Functioning in Young Children with Autistic Disorder

DOI: 10.1155/2013/868396

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Psychophysiology studies of heart rate and heart rate variability can be employed to study regulatory processes in children with autism. The objective of this study was to test for differences in respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA; a measure of heart rate variability) and to examine the relationship between physiologic responses and measures of social behavior. Participants included 2- to 6-year-old children with Autistic Disorder and children without autism. Heart rate and RSA were derived from ECG recordings made during a baseline period and then a stranger approach paradigm. Social and adaptive behavior was assessed by parent report. Groups did not differ in mean heart rate or RSA at baseline or in response to social challenge. However, children with autism were more likely to show a physiologic response to intrusive portions of the stranger approach than to less intrusive portions of this procedure. Nonautistic children were equally likely to respond to intrusive and less intrusive social events. Within the autistic group, physiologic response to the intrusive stranger approach corresponded to higher ratings of social adaptive behaviors. These results suggest that physiologic responses to social challenge may help understand differences in social behavioral outcomes in children with autism. 1. Introduction Past research has suggested that children with autism show disruptions in autonomic responses to environmental stressors [1–5]. However, research has varied in terms of the ages of children included in studies, the nature of stimuli that constituted the observational protocols, and the resulting findings of physiologic responses in comparison to nonautistic individuals. Moreover, while some studies have examined relations between autonomic measures and measures of social and behavioral functioning (e.g., [5]), relatively little research has compared individual differences in physiologic and behavioral characteristics. Examinations of responses in relation to behavioral markers of symptom presentation, syndrome severity, and other aspects of behavioral functioning may help to reconcile some of the inconsistencies found in prior research and has potential implications for developing and refining assessments and treatments of autism. Cardiorespiratory measures, including heart rate, respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), and more general measures of heart rate variability (HRV), have been widely used in studies of child psychopathology, including some research on autism. RSA, a phenomenon defined as periodic variation in heart rate due to respiratory

References

[1]  W. Hirstein, P. Iversen, and V. S. Ramachandran, “Autonomic responses of autistic children to people and objects,” Proceedings of the Royal Society B, vol. 268, no. 1479, pp. 1883–1888, 2001.
[2]  A. Kylli?inen and J. K. Hietanen, “Skin conductance responses to another person's gaze in children with autism,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 36, no. 4, pp. 517–525, 2006.
[3]  T. P. Levine, S. J. Sheinkopf, M. Pescosolido, A. Rodino, G. Elia, and B. Lester, “Physiologic arousal to social stress in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders: a pilot study,” Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, vol. 6, no. 1, pp. 177–183, 2012.
[4]  M. Toichi and Y. Kamio, “Paradoxical autonomic response to mental tasks in autism,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 33, no. 4, pp. 417–426, 2003.
[5]  A. V. Van Hecke, J. Lebow, E. Bal et al., “Electroencephalogram and heart rate regulation to familiar and unfamiliar people in children with autism spectrum disorders,” Child Development, vol. 80, no. 4, pp. 1118–1133, 2009.
[6]  S. W. Porges, “The polyvagal theory: phylogenetic substrates of a social nervous system,” International Journal of Psychophysiology, vol. 42, no. 2, pp. 123–146, 2001.
[7]  M. Althaus, L. J. M. Mulder, G. Mulder, C. C. Aarnoudse, and R. B. Minderaa, “Cardiac adaptivity to attention-demanding tasks in children with a pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified (PDD-NOS),” Biological Psychiatry, vol. 46, no. 6, pp. 799–809, 1999.
[8]  M. Althaus, A. M. Van Roon, L. J. M. Mulder, G. Mulder, C. C. Aarnoudse, and R. B. Minderaa, “Autonomic response patterns observed during the performance of an attention-demanding task in two groups of children with autistic-type difficulties in social adjustment,” Psychophysiology, vol. 41, no. 6, pp. 893–904, 2004.
[9]  C. Kirschbaum, K.-M. Pirke, and D. H. Hellhammer, “The “Trier social stress test”—a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting,” Neuropsychobiology, vol. 28, no. 1-2, pp. 76–81, 1993.
[10]  E. Bal, E. Harden, D. Lamb, A. V. Van Hecke, J. W. Denver, and S. W. Porges, “Emotion recognition in children with autism spectrum disorders: relations to eye gaze and autonomic state,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 40, no. 3, pp. 358–370, 2010.
[11]  R. Corona, C. Dissanayake, S. Arbelle, P. Wellington, and M. Sigman, “Is affect aversive to young children with autism? Behavioral and cardiac responses to experimenter distress,” Child Development, vol. 69, no. 6, pp. 1494–1502, 1998.
[12]  M. Sigman, C. Dissanayake, R. Corona, and M. Espinosa, “Social and cardiac responses of young children with autism,” Autism, vol. 7, no. 2, pp. 205–216, 2003.
[13]  M. A. Patriquin, A. Scarpa, B. H. Friedman, and S. W. Porges, “Respiratory sinus arrhythmia: a marker for positive social functioning and receptive language skills in children with autism spectrum disorders,” Developmental Psychobiology, vol. 55, pp. 101–112, 2013.
[14]  C. Lord, S. Risi, L. Lambrecht et al., “The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 30, no. 3, pp. 205–223, 2000.
[15]  C. D. Elliott, Differential Ability Scales, Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, Tex, USA, 1990.
[16]  N. Bayley, Bayley Scales of Infant Development, Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, Tex, USA, 2nd edition, 1993.
[17]  S. S. Sparrow, D. A. Balla, and D. V. Cicchetti, Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, Minn, USA, 1984.
[18]  G. Bohlin and B. Hagekull, “Stranger wariness and sociability in the early years,” Infant Behavior and Development, vol. 16, no. 1, pp. 53–67, 1993.
[19]  B. M. Lester and M. Peucker, “New technology helps scientists study interface of behavior and physiologal activity,” Hewlett-Packard: Advances for Medicine, vol. 13, pp. 4–6, 1994.
[20]  S. W. Porges, “Respiratory sinus arrhythmia: physiological basis, quantitative methods, and clinical implications,” in Cardiorespiratory and Cardiosomatic Psychophysiology, P. Grossman, K. Janssen, and D. Vaitl, Eds., pp. 101–115, Plenum Press, New York, NY, USA, 1986.
[21]  B. M. Prizant, A. M. Wetherby, E. Rubin, A. C. Laurent, and P. J. Rydell, The SCERTS Model, Paul H. Brookes, Baltimore, Md, USA, 2006.
[22]  M. H. Bornstein and P. E. Suess, “Child and mother cardiac vagal tone: continuity, stability, and concordance across the first 5 years,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 36, no. 1, pp. 54–65, 2000.
[23]  C. B. Mervis and B. P. Klein-Tasman, “Methodological issues in group-matching designs: α levels for control variable comparisons and measurement characteristics of control and target variables,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 34, no. 1, pp. 7–17, 2004.

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133