[1] | Parker AJ (2007) Binocular depth perception and the cerebral cortex. Nature Review Neuroscience 8: 379–391. doi: 10.1038/nrn2131
|
[2] | Bülthoff I, Bülthoff H, Sinha P (1998) Top-down influences on stereoscopic depth-perception. Nature Neuroscience 1: 254–257. doi: 10.1038/699
|
[3] | Gregory RL (1997) Knowledge in perception and illusion. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 352: 1121–1127. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1997.0095
|
[4] | Hill H, Johnston A (2007) The hollow-face illusion: Object-specific knowledge, general assumptions or properties of the stimulus? Perception 36: 199–223. doi: 10.1068/p5523
|
[5] | Cumming BG, DeAngelis GC (2001) The physiology of stereopsis. Annual Review Neuroscience 24: 203–238. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.203
|
[6] | Neri P (2005) A stereoscopic look at visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology 93: 1823–1826. doi: 10.1152/jn.01068.2004
|
[7] | Cumming BG, Parker AJ (1997) Responses of primary visual cortical neurons to binocular disparity without depth perception. Nature 389: 280–283.
|
[8] | Cumming BG, Parker AJ (2000) Local disparity not perceived depth is signaled by binocular neurons in cortical area V1 of the Macaque. The Journal of Neuroscience 20: 4758–4767.
|
[9] | Johansson G (1973) Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception & Psychophysics 14: 201–211. doi: 10.3758/bf03212378
|
[10] | Jackson S, Blake R (2010) Neural integration of information specifying human structure from form, motion, and depth. The Journal of Neuroscience 30: 838–848. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3116-09.2010
|
[11] | Vanrie J, Verfaillie K (2011) On the depth reversibility of point-light actions. Visual Cognition 19: 1158–1190. doi: 10.1080/13506285.2011.614381
|
[12] | Brooks A, Schouten B, Troje NF, Verfaillie K, Blanke O, et al. (2008) Correlated changes in perceptions of the gender and orientation of ambiguous biological motion figures. Current Biology 18: R728–R729. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.06.054
|
[13] | Schouten B, Troje NF, Brooks A, van der Zwan R, Verfaillie K (2010) The facing bias in biological motion perception: Effects of stimulus gender and observer sex. Attention, Perception and Psychophysics 72: 1256–1260. doi: 10.3758/app.72.5.1256
|
[14] | Schouten B, Troje NF, Verfaillie K (2011) The facing bias in biological motion perception: structure, kinematics, and body parts. Attention, Perception and Psychophysics 73: 130–143. doi: 10.3758/s13414-010-0018-1
|
[15] | Vanrie J, Dekeyser M, Verfaillie K (2004) Bistability and biasing effects in the perception of ambiguous point-light walkers. Perception 33: 547–560. doi: 10.1068/p5004
|
[16] | Brainard DH (1997) The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial vision 10: 433–436. doi: 10.1163/156856897x00357
|
[17] | Pelli DG (1997) The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision 10: 437–442. doi: 10.1163/156856897x00366
|
[18] | Troje NF (2002) Decomposing biological motion: A framework for analysis and synthesis of human gait patterns. Journal of Vision 2: 371–387. doi: 10.1167/2.5.2
|
[19] | Cutting JE, Moore C, Morrison R (1988) Masking the motions of human gait. Perception & Psychophysics 44: 339–347. doi: 10.3758/bf03210415
|
[20] | Pagano RR (2012) Understanding Statistics in the Behavioral Sciences: Wadsworth/Cengage Learning.
|
[21] | Doi H, Shinohara K (2012) Bodily movement of approach is detected faster than that of receding. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 19: 858–863. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0284-0
|
[22] | Schouten B, Davila A, Verfaillie K (2013) Further Explorations of the Facing Bias in Biological Motion Perception: Perspective Cues, Observer Sex, and Response Times. Plos One 8.
|
[23] | Bertenthal BI, Pinto J (1994) Global processing of biological motions. Psychological Science 5: 221–225. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00504.x
|
[24] | Chang DH, Troje NF (2009) Characterizing global and local mechanisms in biological motion perception. Journal of Vision 9: 8 1–10.
|
[25] | Backus BT, Fleet DJ, Parker AJ, Heeger DJ (2001) Human cortical activity correlates with stereoscopic depth perception. Journal of Neurophysiology 86: 2054–2068.
|
[26] | Brouwer GJ, van Ee R, Schwarzbach J (2005) Activation in visual cortex correlates with the awareness of stereoscopic depth. The Journal of Neuroscience 25: 10403–10413. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2408-05.2005
|
[27] | Chandrasekaran C, Canon V, Dahmen JC, Kourtzi Z, Welchman AE (2007) Neural correlates of disparity-defined shape discrimination in the human brain. Journal of Neurophysiology 97: 1553–1565. doi: 10.1152/jn.01074.2006
|
[28] | Durand JB, Peeters R, Norman JF, Todd JT, Orban GA (2009) Parietal regions processing visual 3D shape extracted from disparity. Neuroimage 46: 1114–1126. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.023
|
[29] | Georgieva S, Peeters R, Kolster H, Todd JT, Orban GA (2009) The processing of three-dimensional shape from disparity in the human brain. The Journal of Neuroscience 29: 727–742. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4753-08.2009
|
[30] | Preston TJ, Li S, Kourtzi Z, Welchman AE (2008) Multivoxel Pattern Selectivity for Perceptually Relevant Binocular Disparities in the Human Brain. Journal of Neuroscience 28: 11315–11327. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2728-08.2008
|
[31] | Wismeijer DA, van Ee R, Erkelens CJ (2008) Depth cues, rather than perceived depth, govern vergence. Experimental Brain Research 184: 61–70. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1081-2
|
[32] | Masson GS, Busettini C, Miles FA (1997) Vergence eye movements in response to binocular disparity without depth perception. Nature 389: 283–286. doi: 10.1038/38496
|
[33] | Goodale MA, Milner AD (1992) Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci 15: 20–25. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8
|
[34] | Clifford CW, Harris JA (2005) Contextual modulation outside of awareness. Current biology 15: 574–578. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.01.055
|
[35] | He S, MacLeod DI (2001) Orientation-selective adaptation and tilt after-effect from invisible patterns. Nature 411: 473–476.
|
[36] | Rajimehr R (2004) Unconscious orientation processing. Neuron 41: 663–673. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00041-8
|
[37] | Nawrot M, Blake R (1989) Neural integration of information specifying structure from stereopsis and motion. Science 244: 716–718. doi: 10.1126/science.2717948
|
[38] | Jastorff J, Orban GA (2009) Human functional magnetic resonance imaging reveals separation and integration of shape and motion cues in biological motion processing. The Journal of Neuroscience 29: 7315–7329. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4870-08.2009
|
[39] | Sweeny TD, Haroz S, Whitney D (2012) Reference repulsion in the categorical perception of biological motion. Vision Research 64: 26–34. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.05.008
|
[40] | Ban H, Preston TJ, Meeson A, Welchman AE (2012) The integration of motion and disparity cues to depth in dorsal visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience 15: 636–643. doi: 10.1038/nn.3046
|
[41] | Welchman AE, Deubelius A, Conrad V, Bulthoff HH, Kourtzi Z (2005) 3D shape perception from combined depth cues in human visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience 8: 820–827. doi: 10.1038/nn1461
|