[1] | Puga-Gonzalez I, Hildenbrandt H, Hemelrijk CK (2009) Emergent patterns of social affiliation in primates, a model. Plos Computational Biology 5: e1000630 doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000630.
|
[2] | Das M (2000) Conflict management via third parties. post-conflict affiliation of the aggressor. In: Aureli F, de Waal FBM, editors. Natural conflict resolution. Berkeley: University of California Press.
|
[3] | Koski SE, Sterck EHM (2007) Triadic postconflict affiliation in captive chimpanzees: Does consolation console? Anim Behav 73: 133–142. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.04.009
|
[4] | Koski SE, Sterck EHM (2009) Post-conflict third-party affiliation in chimpanzees: What's in it for the third party? Am J Primatol 71: 409–418. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20668
|
[5] | Romero T, Colmenares F, Aureli F (2009) Testing the function of reconciliation and third-party affiliation for aggressors in hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas hamadryas). Am J Primatol 71: 60–69. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20619
|
[6] | Romero T, Castellanos MA, de Waal FBM (2011) Post-conflict affiliation by chimpanzees with aggressors: Other-oriented versus selfish political strategy. PLoS ONE 6: e22173. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0022173
|
[7] | Fraser ON, Aureli F (2008) Reconciliation, consolation and postconflict behavioral specificity in chimpanzees. Am J Primatol 70: 1–10. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20608
|
[8] | Palagi E, Cordoni G, Borgognini Tarli S (2006) Possible roles of consolation in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Am J Phys Anthropol 129: 105–111. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.20242
|
[9] | de Waal FBM, Aureli F (1996) Consolation, reconciliation, and a possible cognitive difference between macaques and chimpanzees. In: Russon AE, Bard KA, Parker ST, editors. Reaching into Thought: The Mind of the Great Apes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 80–110.
|
[10] | Preston SD, de Waal FBM (2002) Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behav Brain Sci 25: 1–72. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x02000018
|
[11] | Cools AKA, van Hout JM, Nelissen MHJ (2007) Canine reconciliation and third-party-initiated postconflict affiliation: Do peacemaking social mechanisms in dogs rival those of higher primates? Ethology 114: 53–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2007.01443.x
|
[12] | Palagi E, Cordoni G (2009) Postconflict third-party affiliation in Canis lupus: Do wolves share similarities with the great apes? Anim Behav 78: 979–986. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.07.017
|
[13] | Cozzi A, Sighieri C, Gazzano A, Nicol CJ, Baragli P (2010) Post-conflict friendly reunion in a permanent group of horses (Equus caballus). Behav Processes 85: 185–190. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.07.007
|
[14] | Seed AM, Clayton NS, Emery NJ (2007) Postconflict third-party affiliation in rooks, Corvus frugilegus. Curr Biol 17: 152–158. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.11.025
|
[15] | Call J, Aureli F, De Waal FBM (2002) Postconflict third-party affiliation in stumptailed macaques. Anim Behav 63: 209–216. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1908
|
[16] | McFarland R, Majolo B (2012) The occurrence and benefits of postconflict bystander affiliation in wild Barbary macaques, Macaca sylvanus. Anim Behav 84: 583–591. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.06.010
|
[17] | de Waal FBM, Luttrell LM (1989) Towards a comparative socioecology of the genus Macaca: different dominance styles in rhesus and stumptail monkeys. Am J Primatol 19: 83–109. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350190203
|
[18] | Thierry B, Aureli F (2006) Barbary but not barbarian: Social relations in a tolerant macaque. In: Hodges JK, Cortes J, editors. The Barbary macaque: biology, management and conservation. Nottingham: Nottingham University Press. pp. 29–45.
|
[19] | Hemelrijk CK, Puga-Gonzalez I (2012) An individual-oriented model on the emergence of support in fights, its reciprocation and exchange. PLoS ONE 7: e37271. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0037271
|
[20] | de Waal FBM, Luttrell LM (1988) Mechanisms of social reciprocity in three primate species: Symmetrical relationship characteristics or cognition? Ethol Sociobiol 9: 101–118. doi: 10.1016/0162-3095(88)90016-7
|
[21] | Petit O, Thierry B (1994) Aggressive and peaceful interventions in conflicts in Tonkean macaques. Anim Behav 48: 1427–1436. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1994.1378
|
[22] | Demaria C, Thierry B (2001) A comparative study of reconciliation in rhesus and Tonkean macaques. Behaviour 138: 397–410. doi: 10.1163/15685390152032514
|
[23] | De Marco A, Cozzolino R, Dessi-Fulgheri F, Thierry B (2010) Conflicts induce affiliative interactions among bystanders in a tolerant species of macaque (Macaca tonkeana). Anim Behav 80: 197–203. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.04.016
|
[24] | De Marco A, Cozzolino R, Dessi-Fulgheri F, Thierry B (2011) Collective arousal when reuniting after temporary separation in Tonkean macaques. Am J Phys Anthropol 146: 457–464. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.21606
|
[25] | Romero T, de Waal FBM (2010) Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) consolation: Third-party identity as a window on possible function. J Comp Psychol 124: 278–286. doi: 10.1037/a0019144
|
[26] | Fraser O, Stahl D, Aureli F (2008) Stress reduction through consolation in chimpanzees. PNAS 105: 8557–8562. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0804141105
|
[27] | Judge PG, Mullen SH (2005) Quadratic postconflict affiliation among bystanders in a Hamadryas baboon group. Anim Behav 69: 1345–1355. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.08.016
|
[28] | Romero T, Colmenares F, Aureli F (2008) Postconflict affiliation of aggressors in Papio hamadryas. Int J Primatol 29: 1591–1606. doi: 10.1007/s10764-008-9315-0
|
[29] | Clay Z, de Waal FBM (2013) Bonobos respond to distress in others: Consolation across the age spectrum. PLoS ONE 8: e55206. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055206
|
[30] | Aureli F, Schaffner CM (2002) Relationship assessment through emotional mediation. Behaviour 139: 393–420. doi: 10.1163/156853902760102726
|
[31] | Evers E, de Vries H, Spruijt BM, Sterck EHM (2012) Look before you leap - individual variation in social vigilance shapes socio-spatial group properties in an agent-based model. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 66: 931–945. doi: 10.1007/s00265-012-1342-3
|
[32] | Evers E, de Vries H, Spruijt BM, Sterck EHM (2011) Better safe than sorry - socio-spatial group structure emerges from individual variation in fleeing, avoidance or velocity in an agent-based model. PLoS ONE 6: e26189. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026189
|
[33] | Butovskaya M (2004) Social space and degrees of freedom. In: Thierry B, Singh M, Kaumanns W, editors. Macaque Societies: a Model for the Study of Social Organization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 182–185.
|
[34] | Thierry B (1990) Feedback loop between kinship and dominance: The macaque model. J Theor Biol 145: 511–521. doi: 10.1016/s0022-5193(05)80485-0
|
[35] | Hemelrijk CK (1999) An individual-oriented model on the emergence of despotic and egalitarian societies. P Roy Soc Lond B Bio 266: 361–369. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1999.0646
|
[36] | Hemelrijk CK (2000) Towards the integration of social dominance and spatial structure. Anim Behav 59: 1035–1048. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1400
|
[37] | Hemelrijk CK, Wantia J, Isler K (2008) Female dominance over males in primates: Self-organisation and sexual dimorphism. PLoS ONE 3: e2678. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002678
|
[38] | Thierry B (2004) Social epigenesis. In: Thierry B, Singh M, Kaumanns W, editors. Macaque societies: A model for the study of social organisation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 267–289.
|
[39] | Thierry B (1985) Patterns of agonistic interactions in three species of macaque (Macaca mulatta, M. fascicularis, M. tonkeana). Aggressive Behav 11: 223–233. doi: 10.1002/1098-2337(1985)11:3<223::aid-ab2480110305>3.0.co;2-a
|
[40] | de Waal FBM, Yoshihara D (1983) Reconciliation and redirected affection in rhesus monkeys. Behaviour 85: 224–241. doi: 10.1163/156853983x00237
|
[41] | Thierry B, Demaria C, Preuschoft S, Desportes C (1989) Structural convergence between silent bared-teeth display and relaxed open-mouth display in the Tonkean macaque (Macaca tonkeana). Folia Primatol 52: 178–184. doi: 10.1159/000156396
|
[42] | Veenema HC, Das M, Aureli F (1994) Methodological improvements for the study of reconciliation. Behav Process 31: 29–38. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(94)90035-3
|
[43] | Hemelrijk CK, Wantia J, Gygax L (2005) The construction of dominance order: Comparing performance of five different methods using an individual-based model. Behaviour 142: 1043–1064. doi: 10.1163/156853905774405290
|
[44] | Hemelrijk CK (1990) Models of, and tests for, reciprocity, unidirectional and other social interaction patterns at a group level. Anim Behav 39: 1013–1029. doi: 10.1016/s0003-3472(05)80775-4
|
[45] | Hsu Y, Earley RL, Wolf LL (2006) Modulation of aggressive behaviour by fighting experience: Mechanisms and contest outcomes. Biol Rev 81: 33–74. doi: 10.1017/s146479310500686x
|
[46] | Hogeweg P (1988) MIRROR beyond MIRROR, puddles of LIFE. In: Langton C, editor. Artificial life, SFI studies in the sciences of complexity. Redwood City, California: Adisson-Wesley Publishing Company. pp. 297–316.
|
[47] | Setchell JM, Smith T, Wickings EJ, Knapp LA (2008) Social correlates of testosterone and ornamentation in male mandrills. Horm Behav 54: 365–372. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.05.004
|
[48] | Barchas PR, Mendoza SD (1984) Emergent hierarchical relationships in rhesus macaques: An application of chase's model. In: Barchas PR, editor. Social hierarchies: Essays Towards a Sociophysiological Perspective. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. pp. 81–95.
|
[49] | Silk JB (2002) The form and function of reconciliation in primates. Ann Rev Anthropol 31: 21–44.
|
[50] | Aureli F (1997) Post-conflict anxiety in nonhuman primates: The mediating role of emotion in conflict resolution. Aggressive Behav 23: 315–328. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-2337(1997)23:5<315::aid-ab2>3.0.co;2-h
|
[51] | Das M, Penke Z, van Hooff JARAM (1998) Postconflict affiliation and stress-related behaviour of long-tailed macaque aggressors. Int J Primatol 19: 53–71.
|
[52] | Aureli F, Preston SD, de Waal FBM (1999) Heart rate responses to social interactions in free-moving rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): A pilot study. J Comp Psychol 113: 59–65. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.113.1.59
|
[53] | Shutt K, MacLarnon A, Heistermann M, Semple S (2007) Grooming in Barbary macaques: Better to give than to receive? Biol Lett 3: 231–233. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2007.0052
|
[54] | Graves FC, Wallen K, Maestripieri D (2002) Opioids and attachment in rhesus macaque (Macaca mulatta) abusive mothers. Behav Neurosci 116: 489–493. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.116.3.489
|
[55] | Schino G, Troisi A (1992) Opiate receptor blockade in juvenile macaques: Effect on affiliative interactions with their mothers and group companions. Brain Res 576: 125–130. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)90617-i
|
[56] | Keverne EB, Martensz ND, Tuite B (1989) Beta-endorphin concentrations in cerebrospinal-fluid of monkeys are influenced by grooming relationships. Psychoneuroendocrino 14: 155–161. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(89)90065-6
|
[57] | Hochberg Y (1988) A sharper Bonferroni procedure for multiple tests of significance. Biometrika 75: 800–802. doi: 10.1093/biomet/75.4.800
|