[1] | Dehaene S, Changeux JP (1993) Development of Elementary Numerical Abilities - a Neuronal Model. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 5: 390–407. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1993.5.4.390
|
[2] | Fias W, Fischer MH (2005) Spatial representation of number. In: Campbell JID, editor. Handbook of Mathematical Cognition. East Sussex: Psychology Press. pp. 43–54.
|
[3] | Müller D, Schwarz W (2007) Is there an internal association of numbers to hands? The task set influences the nature of the SNARC effect. Memory & Cognition 35: 1151–1161. doi: 10.3758/bf03193485
|
[4] | Fischer M (2003) Spatial representations in number processing–evidence from a pointing task. Visual Cognition 10: 493–508. doi: 10.1080/13506280244000186
|
[5] | Shaki S, Fischer MH (2008) Reading space into numbers: a cross-linguistic comparison of the SNARC effect. Cognition 108: 590–599. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.04.001
|
[6] | Hubbard EM, Piazza M, Pinel P, Dehaene S (2005) Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6: 435–448. doi: 10.1038/nrn1684
|
[7] | Fias W, van Dijck J-P, Gevers W (2011) How is Number Associated with Space? The Role of Working Memory. In: Brannon SDaE, editor. Space, Time and Number in the brain: Elsevier. pp. 133–148.
|
[8] | Dehaene S, Bossini S, Giraux P (1993) The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 122: 371–396.
|
[9] | Chen Q, Verguts T (2010) Beyond the mental number line: A neural network model of number–space interactions. Cognitive Psychology 60: 218–240. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.01.001
|
[10] | Casasanto D, Boroditsky L (2008) Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition 106: 579–593. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.004
|
[11] | Dehaene S, Cohen L (2007) Cultural Recycling of Cortical Maps. Neuron 56: 384–398. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.004
|
[12] | van Dijck JP, Abrahamse EL, Majerus S, Fias W (2013) Spatial attention interacts with serial-order retrieval from verbal working memory. Psychol Sci 24: 1854–1859. doi: 10.1177/0956797613479610
|
[13] | van Dijck J-P, Fias W (2011) A working memory account for spatial–numerical associations. Cognition 119: 114–119. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.12.013
|
[14] | Gevers W, Reynvoet B, Fias W (2003) The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized. Cognition 87: B87–B95. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00234-2
|
[15] | Maass A, Russo A (2003) Directional Bias in the Mental Representation of Spatial Events: Nature or Culture? Psychological Science 14: 296–301. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.14421
|
[16] | Tomonaga MMT, Itakura S (1993) Teaching ordinals to a cardinal trained chimpanzee. Primate Research 9: 67–77. doi: 10.2354/psj.9.2_67
|
[17] | Inoue S, Matsuzawa T (2007) Working memory of numerals in chimpanzees. Curr Biol 17: R1004–1005. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.10.027
|
[18] | Matsuzawa T (2009) Symbolic representation of number in chimpanzees. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 19: 92–98. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.04.007
|
[19] | Matsuzawa T (1985) Use of Numbers by a Chimpanzee. Nature 315: 57–59. doi: 10.1038/315057a0
|
[20] | Matsuzawa T (2003) The Ai project: historical and ecological contexts. Animal Cognition 6: 199–211. doi: 10.1007/s10071-003-0199-2
|
[21] | Matsuzawa T (2006) Sociocognitive Development in Chimpanzees: A synthesis of Laboratory Work and Fieldwork. In: Matsuzawa T, Tomonaga, M, Tanaka, M., editor. Cognitive Development in Chimpanzees. Tokyo: Springer. pp. 3–33.
|
[22] | Matsuzawa T, Tomonaga M, Tanaka M (2006) Cognitive Development in Chimpanzees: Springer.
|
[23] | Kawai N, Matsuzawa T (2000) Numerical memory span in a chimpanzee. Nature 403: 39–40. doi: 10.1038/47405
|
[24] | Biro D, Matsuzawa T (2001) Use of numerical symbols by the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): Cardinals, ordinals, and the introduction of zero. Animal Cognition 4: 193–199. doi: 10.1007/s100710100086
|