[1] | Wimmer H, Perner J (1983) Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition 13: 103–1. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5
|
[2] | Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U (1985) Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition 21: 37–46. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8
|
[3] | Birch SAJ, Bloom P (2003) Children are cursed: An asymmetric bias in mental-state attribution. Psychol Sci 3: 283–286. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.03436
|
[4] | Bernstein DM, Atance C, Loftus GR, Meltzoff AN (2004) We saw it all along: Visual hindsight bias in children and adults. Psychol Sci 15: 264–267. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00663.x
|
[5] | Camerer C, Loewenstein G, Weber M (1989) The curse of knowledge in economic settings: An experimental analysis. J Polit Econ 97: 1232–1254. doi: 10.1086/261651
|
[6] | Birch SAJ, Bloom P (2007) The Curse of Knowledge in Reasoning About False Belief. Psychol Sci 18: 382–386. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01909.x
|
[7] | Pohl R (1998) The effects of feedback source and plausibility of hindsight bias. Eur J Cogn Psychol 10: 191–212. doi: 10.1080/713752272
|
[8] | Wasserman D, Lempert RO, Hastie R (1990) Hindsight and causality. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 17: 30–35. doi: 10.1177/0146167291171005
|
[9] | Apperly IA, Back E, Samson D, France L (2008) The cost of thinking about false beliefs: Evidence from adults’ performance on a non-inferential theory of mind task. Cognition 106: 1093–1108. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.05.005
|
[10] | Converse BA, Lin S, Keysar B, Epley N (2008) In the mood to get over yourself: Mood affects theory-of-mind use. Emotion 8: 725–730. doi: 10.1037/a0013283
|
[11] | Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang A-G (2009) Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods 41: 1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/brm.41.4.1149
|
[12] | Paolacci G, Chandler J, Ipeirotis P (2010) Running experiments on amazon mechanical turk. Judgm Decis Mak 5: 411–419.
|
[13] | Buhrmester M, Kwang T, Gosling SD (2011) Amazon's Mechanical Turk A New Source of Inexpensive, Yet High-Quality, Data? Persp Psychol Sci 6: 3–5. doi: 10.1177/1745691610393980
|
[14] | Germine L, Nakayama K, Duchaine BC, Chabris CF, Chatterjee G, et al. (2012) Is the Web as good as the lab? Comparable performance from Web and lab in cognitive/perceptual experiments. Psychon Bull Rev 19: 847–857. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0296-9
|
[15] | Gardner WL, Gabriel S, Lee A (1999) “I” value freedom but “we” value relationships: Self-construal priming mirrors cultural differences in judgment. Psychol Sci 10: 321–326. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00162
|
[16] | Trafimow D, Triandis H, Goto S (1991) Some tests of the distinction between the private self and the collective self. J Pers Soc Psychol 60: 649–655. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.60.5.649
|
[17] | Keysar B, Lin S, Barr DJ (2003) Limits on theory of mind use in adults. Cognition 89: 25–41. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(03)00064-7
|
[18] | Simonsohn U (2013) Evaluating Replication Results. Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=2259879 or http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2259879.
|
[19] | Brown-Schmidt S (2009) The role of executive function in perspective taking during online language comprehension. Psychon Bulletin Rev 16: 893–900. doi: 10.3758/pbr.16.5.893
|
[20] | Hanna JE, Tanenhaus MK, Trueswell JC (2003) The effects of common ground and perspective on domains of referential interpretation. J Mem Lang 49: 43–61. doi: 10.1016/s0749-596x(03)00022-6
|