全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...
PLOS ONE  2014 

Fox Squirrels Match Food Assessment and Cache Effort to Value and Scarcity

DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0092892

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Scatter hoarders must allocate time to assess items for caching, and to carry and bury each cache. Such decisions should be driven by economic variables, such as the value of the individual food items, the scarcity of these items, competition for food items and risk of pilferage by conspecifics. The fox squirrel, an obligate scatter-hoarder, assesses cacheable food items using two overt movements, head flicks and paw manipulations. These behaviors allow an examination of squirrel decision processes when storing food for winter survival. We measured wild squirrels' time allocations and frequencies of assessment and investment behaviors during periods of food scarcity (summer) and abundance (fall), giving the squirrels a series of 15 items (alternating five hazelnuts and five peanuts). Assessment and investment per cache increased when resource value was higher (hazelnuts) or resources were scarcer (summer), but decreased as scarcity declined (end of sessions). This is the first study to show that assessment behaviors change in response to factors that indicate daily and seasonal resource abundance, and that these factors may interact in complex ways to affect food storing decisions. Food-storing tree squirrels may be a useful and important model species to understand the complex economic decisions made under natural conditions.

References

[1]  Smith CC, Reichman OJ (1984) The evolution of food caching by birds and mammals. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 15: 329–351. doi: 10.1146/annurev.es.15.110184.001553
[2]  Vander Wall SB (1990) Food Hoarding in Animals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 453.
[3]  Andersson M, Krebs J (1978) On the evolution of hoarding behaviour. Animal Behaviour 26: 707–711. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(78)90137-9
[4]  Stephens DW, Krebs JR (1986) Foraging Theory. Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press. 247.
[5]  Eibl-Eibesfeldt I (1963) Angeborenes und erworbenes im verhalten einiger Sauger. Zeitschrift fuer Tierpsychologie 20: 705–754. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1963.tb01184.x
[6]  Horwich RH (1972) The ontogeny of social behavior in the gray squirrel (Sciurus carolinensis). Berlin: Paul Parey Scientific Publishers. 103.
[7]  Herrnstein RJ (1961) Relative and absolute strength of response as a function of frequency of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 4: 267–272. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1961.4-267
[8]  Valone TJ (2006) Are animals capable of Bayesian updating? An empirical review. Oikos 112: 252–259. doi: 10.1111/j.0030-1299.2006.13465.x
[9]  Preston SD, Jacobs LF (2009) Mechanisms of cache decision making in fox squirrels (Sciurus niger). Journal of Mammalogy 90: 787–795. doi: 10.1644/08-mamm-a-254.1
[10]  Lichti NI (2012) Implications of context-dependent scatterhoarding for seed survival and dispersal in North American oaks (Quercus) [Doctoral Dissertation]: Purdue University.
[11]  Moore JE, McEuen AB, Swihart RK, Contreras TA, Steele MA (2007) Determinants of seed removal distance by scatter-hoarding rodents in deciduous forests. Ecology 88: 2529–2540. doi: 10.1890/07-0247.1
[12]  Wang B, Ye CX, Cannon CH, Chen J (2012) Dissecting the decision making process of scatter-hoarding rodents. Oikos 122: 1027–1034. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0706.2012.20823.x
[13]  Gerber LR, Reichman OJ, Roughgarden J (2004) Food hoarding: Future value in optimal foraging decisions. Ecological Modelling 175: 77–85. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2003.10.022
[14]  Kislalioglu M, Gibson RN (1976) Prey ‘handling time’ and its importance in food selection by the 15-spined stickleback, Spinachia spinachia. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 25: 151–158. doi: 10.1016/0022-0981(76)90016-2
[15]  Langen TA (1999) How western scrub-jays (Aphelocoma californica) select a nut: Effects of the number of options, variation in nut size, and social competition among foragers. Animal Cognition 2: 223–233. doi: 10.1007/s100710050043
[16]  Langen TA, Gibson RM (1998) Sampling and information acquisition by western scrub-jays, Aphelocoma californica. Animal Behaviour 55: 1245–1254. doi: 10.1016/s0003-3472(98)90000-8
[17]  Ligon JD, Martin DJ (1974) Pi?on seed assessment by the pi?on jay, Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus. Animal Behaviour 22: 421–429. doi: 10.1016/s0003-3472(74)80040-0
[18]  Melin AD, Fedigan LM, Hiramatsu C, Hiwatashi T, Parr N, et al. (2009) Fig foraging by dichromatic and trichromatic Cebus capucinus in a tropical dry forest. International Journal of Primatology 30: 753–775. doi: 10.1007/s10764-009-9383-9
[19]  Rockwell C, Gabriel PO, Black JM (2013) Foraging dynamics in Steller's jays: Size and viability of cacheable food items. Animal Behaviour 86: 783–789. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.07.018
[20]  Thompson DC, Thompson PS (1980) Food habits and caching behavior of urban grey squirrels. Canadian Journal of Zoology 58: 701–710. doi: 10.1139/z80-101
[21]  Kraus B (1983) A test of the optimal-density model for seed scatterhoarding. Ecology 64: 608–610. doi: 10.2307/1939982
[22]  Leaver LA, Hopewell L, Caldwell C, Mallarky L (2007) Audience effects on food caching in grey squirrels (Sciurus carolinensis): Evidence for pilferage avoidance strategies. Animal Cognition 10: 23–27. doi: 10.1007/s10071-006-0026-7
[23]  McQuade DB, Williams EH, Eichenbaum HB (1986) Cues used for localizing food by the gray squirrel (Sciurus carolinensis). Ethology 72: 22–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1986.tb00602.x
[24]  Schmidt KA, Ostfeld RS (2008) Eavesdropping squirrels reduce their future value of food under the perceived presence of cache robbers. The American Naturalist 171: 386–393. doi: 10.1086/527497
[25]  Hopewell LJ, Leaver LA (2008) Evidence of social influences on cache-making by grey squirrels (Sciurus carolinensis). Ethology 114: 1061–1068. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2008.01554.x
[26]  Nixon CM, Worley DM, McClain MW (1968) Food habits of squirrels in southeast Ohio. The Journal of Wildlife Management 32: 294–305. doi: 10.2307/3798974
[27]  Kotler BP, Brown JS, Hickey M (1999) Food storability and the foraging behavior of fox squirrels (Sciurus niger). The American Midland Naturalist 142: 77–86. doi: 10.1674/0003-0031(1999)142[0077:fsatfb]2.0.co;2
[28]  Goodrum PD (1972) Adult fox squirrel weights in eastern Texas. The Journal of Wildlife Management: 159–161.
[29]  Short HL, Duke WB (1971) Seasonal food consumption and body weights of captive tree squirrels. The Journal of Wildlife Management 35: 435–439. doi: 10.2307/3799694
[30]  Nixon CM, Hansen LP, Havera SP (1991) Growth patterns of fox squirrels in East-central Illinois. American Midland Naturalist 125: 168–172. doi: 10.2307/2426380
[31]  Lewis AR (1982) Selection of nuts by gray squirrels and optimal foraging theory. American Midland Naturalist 107: 250–257. doi: 10.2307/2425376
[32]  Brown JS, Morgan RA, Dow BD (1992) Patch use under predation risk: H. A test with fox squirrels, Sciurus niger. Annales Zoologici Fennici 29: 1–318.
[33]  Stapanian MA, Smith CC (1984) Density-dependent survival of scatterhoarded nuts: An experimental approach. Ecology 65: 1387–1396. doi: 10.2307/1939119
[34]  Stapanian MA, Smith CC (1978) A model for seed scatterhoarding: Coevolution of fox squirrels and black walnuts. Ecology 59: 884–896. doi: 10.2307/1938541
[35]  Steele MA, Contreras TA, Hadj-Chikh LZ, Agosta SJ, Smallwood PD, et al. (2014) Do scatter hoarders trade off increased predation risks for lower rates of cache pilferage? Behavioral Ecology 25: 206–215. doi: 10.1093/beheco/art107
[36]  Steele MA, Halkin SL, Smallwood PD, McKenna TJ, Mitsopoulos K, et al. (2008) Cache protection strategies of a scatter-hoarding rodent: Do tree squirrels engage in behavioural deception? Animal Behaviour 75: 705–714. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.07.026
[37]  Jacobs LF, Shiflett MW (1999) Spatial orientation on a vertical maze in free-ranging fox squirrels (Sciurus niger). Journal of Comparative Psychology 113: 116–127. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.113.2.116
[38]  Waisman AS, Jacobs LF (2008) Flexibility of cue use in the fox squirrel (Sciurus niger). Animal Cognition 11: 625–636. doi: 10.1007/s10071-008-0152-5
[39]  Cockrell RA, Warnke F (1976) Trees of the Berkeley campus: Division of Agricultural Sciences, University of California.
[40]  Fowells HA, Schubert GH (1956) Seed crops of forest trees in the pine region of California (No. 1150): US Dept. of Agriculture.
[41]  McDonald PM (1969) Silvical characteristics of California black oak (Quercus kelloggii Newb.) (Vol. 53): Pacific Southwest Forest and Range Experiment Station.
[42]  Koenig WD, Knops J, Carmen WJ, Pearse I (2008–2013) California acorn survey.
[43]  U.S. Department of Agriculture (2012) USDA national nutrient database for standard reference, release 25.
[44]  Bolker BM, Brooks ME, Clark CJ, Geange SW, Poulsen JR, et al. (2009) Generalized linear mixed models: A practical guide for ecology and evolution. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 24: 127–135. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2008.10.008
[45]  Skaug H, Fournier D, Nielsen A (2006) glmmADMB: Generalized linear mixed models using AD Model Builder. Available: http://glmmadmb.r-forge.r-project.org/.
[46]  Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B (2011) lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. Available: http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/l?me4/index.html.
[47]  Coxe S, West SG, Aiken LS (2009) The analysis of count data: A gentle introduction to Poisson regression and its alternatives. Journal of Personality Assessment 91: 121–136. doi: 10.1080/00223890802634175
[48]  Loewenstein G (1987) Anticipation and the valuation of delayed consumption. The Economic Journal 97: 666–684. doi: 10.2307/2232929
[49]  Leaver L, Hopewell L, Caldwell C, Mallarky L (2007) Audience effects on food caching in grey squirrels (Sciurus carolinensis): Evidence for pilferage avoidance strategies. Animal Cognition 10: 23–27. doi: 10.1007/s10071-006-0026-7
[50]  Hadj-Chikh LZ, Steele MA, Smallwood PD (1996) Caching decisions by grey squirrels: A test of the handling time and perishability hypotheses. Animal Behaviour 52: 941–948. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1996.0242
[51]  McClure SM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD (2004) Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science 306: 503–507. doi: 10.1126/science.1100907
[52]  Waisman AS, Lucas CG, Griffiths TL, Jacobs LF (2011) A Bayesian model of navigation in squirrels. Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.
[53]  Wood M (1998) Socio-economic status, delay of gratification, and impulse buying. Journal of Economic Psychology 19: 295–320. doi: 10.1016/s0167-4870(98)00009-9
[54]  Lavenex P, Steele MA, Jacobs LF (2000) Sex differences, but no seasonal variations in the hippocampus of food-caching squirrels: A stereological study. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 425: 152–166. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20000911)425:1<152::aid-cne13>3.3.co;2-p
[55]  Muul I (1969) Photoperiod and reproduction in flying squirrels, Glaucomys volans. Journal of Mammalogy: 542–549.
[56]  Burger DK, Saucier JM, Iwaniuk AN, Saucier DM (2013) Seasonal and sex differences in the hippocampus of a wild rodent. Behavioural Brain Research 236: 131–138. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.08.044

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133