全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...
PLOS ONE  2014 

Crossing the Hands Increases Illusory Self-Touch

DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0094008

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Manipulation of hand posture, such as crossing the hands, has been frequently used to study how the body and its immediately surrounding space are represented in the brain. Abundant data show that crossed arms posture impairs remapping of tactile stimuli from somatotopic to external space reference frame and deteriorates performance on several tactile processing tasks. Here we investigated how impaired tactile remapping affects the illusory self-touch, induced by the non-visual variant of the rubber hand illusion (RHI) paradigm. In this paradigm blindfolded participants (Experiment 1) had their hands either uncrossed or crossed over the body midline. The strength of illusory self-touch was measured with questionnaire ratings and proprioceptive drift. Our results showed that, during synchronous tactile stimulation, the strength of illusory self-touch increased when hands were crossed compared to the uncrossed posture. Follow-up experiments showed that the increase in illusion strength was not related to unfamiliar hand position (Experiment 2) and that it was equally strengthened regardless of where in the peripersonal space the hands were crossed (Experiment 3). However, while the boosting effect of crossing the hands was evident from subjective ratings, the proprioceptive drift was not modulated by crossed posture. Finally, in contrast to the illusion increase in the non-visual RHI, the crossed hand postures did not alter illusory ownership or proprioceptive drift in the classical, visuo-tactile version of RHI (Experiment 4). We argue that the increase in illusory self-touch is related to misalignment of somatotopic and external reference frames and consequently inadequate tactile-proprioceptive integration, leading to re-weighting of the tactile and proprioceptive signals.The present study not only shows that illusory self-touch can be induced by crossing the hands, but importantly, that this posture is associated with a stronger illusion.

References

[1]  Blanke O, Metzinger T (2009) Full-body illusions and minimal phenomenal selfhood. Trends Cogn Sci 13: 7–13. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.10.003
[2]  Damasio A (1999) The Feeling of What Happens. London: Random House.
[3]  Gallagher S (2005) How the body shapes the mind. Oxford; New York: Clarendon Press. 284 p. p.
[4]  Berlucchi G, Aglioti S (1997) The body in the brain: neural bases of corporeal awareness. Trends in Neurosciences 20: 560–564. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(97)01136-3
[5]  Haggard P, Taylor-Clarke M, Kennett S (2003) Tactile perception, cortical representation and the bodily self. Current Biology 13: R170–R173. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(03)00115-5
[6]  Jeannerod M (2007) Being oneself. Journal of Physiology-Paris 101: 161–168. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2007.11.005
[7]  Metzinger T, Rahul B, Bikas KC (2007) Empirical perspectives from the self-model theory of subjectivity: a brief summary with examples. Progress in Brain Research: Elsevier. pp. 215–245, 273–278.
[8]  Serino A, Haggard P (2010) Touch and the body. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 34: 224–236. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.04.004
[9]  Schütz-Bosbach S, Musil JJ, Haggard P (2009) Touchant-touché: The role of self-touch in the representation of body structure. Consciousness and cognition 18: 2–11. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.08.003
[10]  Kammers MPM, de Vignemont F, Haggard P (2010) Cooling the Thermal Grill Illusion through Self-Touch. Current Biology 20: 1819–1822. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.08.038
[11]  van Stralen H, van Zandvoort M, Dijkerman H (2011) The role of self-touch in somatosensory and body representation disorders after stroke. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 366: 3142–3152. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0163
[12]  Salomon R, Lim M, Kannape O, Llobera J, Blanke O (2013) “Self pop-out”: agency enhances self-recognition in visual search. Experimental Brain Research 228: 173–181. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3549-6
[13]  Merleau-Ponty M (2002) Phenomenology of Perception: An Introduction. Smith C, translator: Taylor & Francis Group (original work published in 1945).
[14]  Medina J, Coslett HB (2010) From maps to form to space: Touch and the body schema. Neuropsychologia 48: 645–654. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.08.017
[15]  Yamamoto S, Kitazawa S (2001) Reversal of subjective temporal order due to arm crossing. Nat Neurosci 4: 759–765.
[16]  de Haan AM, Anema HA, Dijkerman HC (2012) Fingers Crossed! An Investigation of Somatotopic Representations Using Spatial Directional Judgements. PLoS One 7: e45408. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0045408
[17]  Longo MR, Aza?ón E, Haggard P (2010) More than skin deep: Body representation beyond primary somatosensory cortex. Neuropsychologia 48: 655–668. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.08.022
[18]  Sekine T, Mogi K (2009) Distinct neural processes of bodily awareness in crossed fingers illusion. NeuroReport 20: : 467–472 410.1097/WNR.1090b1013e3283277087.
[19]  Nakamura A, Yamada T, Goto A, Kato T, Ito K, et al. (1998) Somatosensory Homunculus as Drawn by MEG. NeuroImage 7: 377–386. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0332
[20]  Penfield W, Boldrey E (1937) Somatic motor and sensory representation in the cerebral cortex of man as studied by electrical stimulation. Brain 60: 389–443. doi: 10.1093/brain/60.4.389
[21]  Narici L, Modena I, Opsomer RJ, Pizzella V, Romani GL, et al. (1991) Neuromagnetic somatosensory homunculus: a non-invasive approach in humans. Neuroscience Letters 121: 51–54. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(91)90647-c
[22]  Kurth R, Villringer K, Curio G, Wolf K-J, Krause T, et al. (2000) fMRI shows multiple somatotopic digit representations in human primary somatosensory cortex. NeuroReport 11: 1487–1491. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200005150-00025
[23]  Oldfield SR, Phillips JR (1983) The spatial characteristics of tactile form perception. Perception 12: 615–626. doi: 10.1068/p120615
[24]  Pouget A, Deneve S, Duhamel J-R (2002) A computational perspective on the neural basis of multisensory spatial representations. Nat Rev Neurosci 3: 741–747. doi: 10.1038/nrn914
[25]  Shore DI, Spry E, Spence C (2002) Confusing the mind by crossing the hands. Cognitive Brain Research 14: 153–163. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(02)00070-8
[26]  Matsumoto E, Misaki M, Miyauchi S (2004) Neural mechanisms of spatial stimulus–response compatibility: the effect of crossed-hand position. Experimental Brain Research 158: 9–17. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-1872-7
[27]  Eimer M, Forster B, Van Velzen J (2003) Anterior and posterior attentional control systems use different spatial reference frames: ERP evidence from covert tactile-spatial orienting. Psychophysiology 40: 924–933. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.00110
[28]  Holmes NP, Sanabria D, Calvert GA, Spence C (2006) Multisensory interactions follow the hands across the midline: Evidence from a non-spatial visual-tactile congruency task. Brain Research 1077: 108–115. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.11.010
[29]  Spence C, Pavani F, Driver J (2004) Spatial constraints on visual-tactile cross-modal distractor congruency effects. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 4: 148–169. doi: 10.3758/cabn.4.2.148
[30]  Botvinick M, Cohen J (1998) Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see. Nature 391: 756.
[31]  Ehrsson HH, Spence C, Passingham RE (2004) That's My Hand! Activity in Premotor Cortex Reflects Feeling of Ownership of a Limb. Science 305: 875–877. doi: 10.1126/science.1097011
[32]  Lloyd DM (2007) Spatial limits on referred touch to an alien limb may reflect boundaries of visuo-tactile peripersonal space surrounding the hand. Brain and Cognition 64: 104–109. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2006.09.013
[33]  Aimola Davies AM, White RC, Thew G, Aimola NM, Davies M (2010) Visual capture of action, experience of ownership, and the illusion of self-touch: a new rubber hand paradigm. Perception 39: 830–838. doi: 10.1068/p6610
[34]  Armel KC, Ramachandran VS (2003) Projecting sensations to external objects: evidence from skin conductance response. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 270: 1499–1506. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2364
[35]  Pavani F, Spence C, Driver J (2000) Visual capture of touch: out-of-the-body experiences with rubber gloves. Psychol Sci 11: 353–359. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00270
[36]  Tsakiris M, Haggard P (2005) The Rubber Hand Illusion Revisited: Visuotactile Integration and Self-Attribution. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 31: 80–91. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.1.80
[37]  Farnè A, Pavani F, Meneghello F, Làdavas E (2000) Left tactile extinction following visual stimulation of a rubber hand. Brain 123: 2350–2360. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.11.2350
[38]  Costantini M, Haggard P (2007) The rubber hand illusion: Sensitivity and reference frame for body ownership. Consciousness and Cognition 16: 229–240. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2007.01.001
[39]  Haans A, Ijsselsteijn WA, de Kort YAW (2008) The effect of similarities in skin texture and hand shape on perceived ownership of a fake limb. Body Image 5: 389–394. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2008.04.003
[40]  Graziano MSA, Cooke DF, Taylor CSR (2000) Coding the Location of the Arm by Sight. Science 290: 1782–1786. doi: 10.1126/science.290.5497.1782
[41]  Ehrsson HH, Holmes NP, Passingham RE (2005) Touching a Rubber Hand: Feeling of Body Ownership Is Associated with Activity in Multisensory Brain Areas. J Neurosci 25: 10564–10573. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0800-05.2005
[42]  Aimola Davies AM, White RC, Davies M (2013) Spatial limits on the nonvisual self-touch illusion and the visual rubber hand illusion: Subjective experience of the illusion and proprioceptive drift. Consciousness and Cognition 22: 613–636. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.03.006
[43]  White RC, Aimola Davies AM, Davies M (2011) Two hands are better than one: A new assessment method and a new interpretation of the non-visual illusion of self-touch. Consciousness and Cognition 20: 956–964. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.03.021
[44]  Dieguez S, Mercier MR, Newby N, Blanke O (2009) Feeling numbness for someone else's finger. Current Biology 19: R1108–R1109. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.10.055
[45]  Aza?ón E, Soto-Faraco S (2008) Changing Reference Frames during the Encoding of Tactile Events. Current Biology 18: 1044–1049. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.06.045
[46]  Moscovitch M, Behrmann M (1994) Coding of Spatial Information in the Somatosensory System: Evidence from Patients with Neglect following Parietal Lobe Damage. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 6: 151–155. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1994.6.2.151
[47]  Aglioti S, Smania N, Peru A (1999) Frames of Reference for Mapping Tactile Stimuli in Brain-Damaged Patients. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 11: 67–79. doi: 10.1162/089892999563256
[48]  Holmes N, Spence C (2004) The body schema and multisensory representation(s) of peripersonal space. Cognitive Processing 5: 94–105. doi: 10.1007/s10339-004-0013-3
[49]  Makin TR, Holmes NP, Ehrsson HH (2008) On the other hand: Dummy hands and peripersonal space. Behavioural Brain Research 191: 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.02.041
[50]  Graziano MSA (1999) Where is my arm? The relative role of vision and proprioception in the neuronal representation of limb position. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 96: 10418–10421. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.18.10418
[51]  Ernst MO, Bülthoff HH (2004) Merging the senses into a robust percept. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8: 162–169. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.002
[52]  Block HJ, Bastian AJ (2011) Sensory weighting and realignment: independent compensatory processes. Journal of Neurophysiology 106: 59–70. doi: 10.1152/jn.00641.2010
[53]  Lopez C, Lenggenhager B, Blanke O (2010) How vestibular stimulation interacts with illusory hand ownership. Consciousness and Cognition 19: 33–47. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2009.12.003
[54]  de Vignemont F, Ehrsson HH, Haggard P (2005) Bodily Illusions Modulate Tactile Perception. Current Biology 15: 1286–1290. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.06.067
[55]  Macaluso E, Maravita A (2010) The representation of space near the body through touch and vision. Neuropsychologia 48: 782–795. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.010
[56]  Hagura N, Takei T, Hirose S, Aramaki Y, Matsumura M, et al. (2007) Activity in the Posterior Parietal Cortex Mediates Visual Dominance over Kinesthesia. The Journal of Neuroscience 27: 7047–7053. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0970-07.2007
[57]  Ramachandran VS, Hirstein W (1998) The perception of phantom limbs. The D. O. Hebb lecture. Brain 121: 1603–1630. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.9.1603
[58]  Petkova VI, Zetterberg H, Ehrsson HH (2012) Rubber Hands Feel Touch, but Not in Blind Individuals. PLoS ONE 7: e35912. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0035912
[59]  Bolognini N, Maravita A (2007) Proprioceptive Alignment of Visual and Somatosensory Maps in the Posterior Parietal Cortex. Current Biology 17: 1890–1895. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.09.057
[60]  R?der B, R?sler F, Spence C (2004) Early Vision Impairs Tactile Perception in the Blind. Current Biology 14: 121–124. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2003.12.054
[61]  Bremner AJ, Mareschal D, Lloyd-Fox S, Spence C (2008) Spatial Localization of Touch in the First Year of Life: Early Influence of a Visual Spatial Code and the Development of Remapping Across Changes in Limb Position. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 137: 149–162.
[62]  Grüsser OJ (1983) Multimodal structure of the extrapersonal space. In: Hein A, Jeannerod M, editors. Spatially oriented behavior. New York, Berlin, Heidelberg, Tokyo: Springer. pp. 327–352.
[63]  Wada M, Yamamoto S, Kitazawa S (2004) Effects of handedness on tactile temporal order judgment. Neuropsychologia 42: 1887–1895. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.05.009
[64]  Aristotle (n.d./2007) The Metaphysics. Mineola, NY: Dover Publications.
[65]  Rivers WHR (1894) A Modification of Aristotle's Experiment. Mind 3: 583–584. doi: 10.1093/mind/iii.12.583-b
[66]  Benedetti F (1985) Processing of tactile spatial information with crossed fingers. Journal of experimental psychology 11: 517–525. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.11.4.517
[67]  Kóbor I, Füredi L, Kovács G, Spence C, Vidnyánszky Z (2006) Back-to-front: Improved tactile discrimination performance in the space you cannot see. Neuroscience Letters 400: 163–167. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.02.037
[68]  van Beers RJ, Sittig AC, Denier van der Gon JJ (1998) The precision of proprioceptive position sense. Experimental Brain Research 122: 367–377. doi: 10.1007/s002210050525
[69]  Peru A, Moro V, Sattibaldi L, Morgant J, Aglioti S (2006) Gravitational influences on reference frames for mapping somatic stimuli in brain-damaged patients. Experimental Brain Research 169: 145–152. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-0132-9
[70]  Graziano MSA, Gross CG (1998) Spatial maps for the control of movement. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 8: 195–201. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(98)80140-2
[71]  White, Davies AMA, Halleen TJ, Davies M (2010) Tactile expectations and the perception of self-touch: An investigation using the rubber hand paradigm. Consciousness and Cognition 19: 505–519. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2009.08.003
[72]  Sigmundsson H, Haga M, Hopkins B (2007) Sex Differences in Perception: Exploring the Integration of Sensory Information with Respect to Vision and Proprioception. Sex Roles 57: 181–186. doi: 10.1007/s11199-007-9228-y
[73]  von Hofsten C, R?sblad B (1988) The integration of sensory information in the development of precise manual pointing. Neuropsychologia 26: 805–821. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(88)90051-6
[74]  Cadieux ML, Whitworth K, Shore DI (2011) Rubber hands do not cross the midline. Neuroscience Letters 504: 191–194. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.09.010

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133