The high failure rate of new market introductions, despite initial successful testing with traditional sensory and consumer tests, necessitates the development of other tests. This study explored the ability of selected physiological and behavioral measures of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) to distinguish between repeated exposures to foods from a single category (breakfast drinks) and with similar liking ratings. In this within-subject study 19 healthy young adults sipped from five breakfast drinks, each presented five times, while ANS responses (heart rate, skin conductance response and skin temperature), facial expressions, liking, and intensities were recorded. The results showed that liking was associated with increased heart rate and skin temperature, and more neutral facial expressions. Intensity was associated with reduced heart rate and skin temperature, more neutral expressions and more negative expressions of sadness, anger and surprise. Strongest associations with liking were found after 1 second of tasting, whereas strongest associations with intensity were found after 2 seconds of tasting. Future studies should verify the contribution of the additional information to the prediction of market success.
References
[1]
Rozin P (1999) Preadaptation and the puzzles and properties of pleasure. In: Kahneman D, Diener E, Schwarz N, editors. Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. 109–133.
[2]
Rolls B, Rolls E, Rowe E, Sweeney K (1981) Sensory specific satiety in man. Physiol Behav 27: 137–142. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(81)90310-3
[3]
Frijda N (1999) Emotions and hedonic experience. In: Kahneman D, Diener E, Schwarz N, editors. Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. 190–210.
[4]
King SC, Meiselman HL (2010) Development of a method to measure consumer emotions associated with foods. Food Qual Prefer 21: 168–177. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2009.02.005
[5]
Desmet PMA, Schifferstein HNJ (2008) Sources of positive and negative emotions in food experience. Appetite 50: 290–301. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.08.003
[6]
Steiner J (1973) The gustofacial response: Observation on normal and anencephalic newborn infants. Symp Oral Sens Percept 4: 254–278.
[7]
Steiner JE, Glaser D, Hawilo ME, Berridge KC (2001) Comparative expression of hedonic impact: affective reactions to taste by human infants and other primates. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 25: 53–74. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(00)00051-8
[8]
Maier A (2007) Early variety experiece at weaning predicts hedonic reponse (liking) to new foods by infants. In: Maier A, editor. Influence des pratiques d’allaitement et de sevrage sur l′acceptation de flaveurs nouvelles chez le jeune enfant: variabilité intra-et inter-régionale. Dijon: University of Bourgogne. (Chapter IV).
[9]
Danner L, Sidorkina L, Joechl M, Duerrschmid K (2013) Make a face! Implicit and explicit measurement of facial expressions elicited by orange juices using face reading technology. Food Qual Prefer: (in press). doi:10.1016/j.foodqual.2013.01.004.
[10]
Zeinstra GG, Koelen M, Colindres D, Kok FJ, de Graaf C (2009) Facial expressions in school-aged children are a good indicator of “dislikes”. but not of “likes.” Food Qual Prefer 20: 620–624. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2009.07.002
[11]
Kreibig SD (2010) Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: a review. Biol Psychol 84: 394–421. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010
[12]
Ellsworth PC, Scherer KR (2003) Appraisal processes in emotion. In: Davidson RJ, Scherer KR, Goldsmith HH, editors. Handbook of affective sciences. New York: Oxford University Press. 572–595.
[13]
Aue T, Flykt A, Scherer KR (2007) First evidence for differential and sequential efferent effects of stimulus relevance and goal conduciveness appraisal. Biol Psychol 74: 347–357. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.09.001
[14]
Delplanque S, Grandjean D, Chrea C, Coppin G, Aymard L, et al. (2009) Sequential unfolding of novelty and pleasantness appraisals of odors: evidence from facial electromyography and autonomic reactions. Emotion 9: 316–328. doi: 10.1037/a0015369
[15]
De Wijk RA, Kooijman V, Verhoeven RHG, Holthuysen NTE, de Graaf C (2012) Autonomic nervous system responses on and facial expressions to the sight, smell, and taste of liked and disliked foods. Food Qual Prefer 26: 196–203. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2012.04.015
[16]
Ekman P, Rolls ET, Perrett DI, Ellis HD (1992) Facial expressions of emotion: an old controversy and new findings. London: Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1992.0008
[17]
Kuilenburg H Van, Wiering M, Den Uyl MJ (2005) A model based method for automatic facial expression recognition. In: Gama J, Camacho R, Brazdil P, Jorge A, Torgo L, editors. Machine Learning: ECML 2005. Springer Berlin Heidelberg, Vol. 3720: 194–205. doi: 10.1007/11564096_22
[18]
Gilbert AN, Fridlund AJ, Sabini J (1987) Hedonic and social determinants of facial displays to odors. Chem Senses 12: 355–363. doi: 10.1093/chemse/12.2.355
[19]
Parkinson B (2005) Do facial movements express emotions or communicate motives? Pers Soc Psychol Rev 9: 278–311. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0904_1
[20]
He W, Boesveldt S, de Graaf C, de Wijk RA (2014) Dynamics of autonomic nervous system responses and facial expressions to odors. Front Psychol 5: (in press). doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00110.