[1] | Rayner K, Carlson M, Frazier L (1983) The Interaction of Syntax and Semantics During Sentence Processing - Eye-Movements in the Analysis of Semantically Biased Sentences. J Verb Learn Verb Beh 22: 358–374.
|
[2] | Frazier L, Rayner K (1982) Making and correcting errors during sentence comprehension: Eye movements in the analysis of structurally ambiguous sentences. Cogn Psychol 14: 178–210.
|
[3] | Crain S, Steedman M (1985) On not being led up the garden path: The use of context by the psychological syntax processor. In: Dowty DR, Karttunen L, Zwicky AM, editors. Natural language parsing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 320–354.
|
[4] | Taraban R, McClelland JL (1988) Constituent attachment and thematic role assignment in sentence processing: Influences of content-based expectations. J Mem Lang 27: 597–632.
|
[5] | Trueswell JC, Tanenhaus MK, Garnsey SM (1994) Semantic Influences On Parsing: Use of Thematic Role Information in Syntactic Ambiguity Resolution. J Mem Lang 33: 285–318.
|
[6] | Gibson E (1998) Linguistic complexity: locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition 68: 1–76.
|
[7] | MacDonald M, Pearlmutter N, Seidenberg M (1994) Lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychol Rev 101: 676–703.
|
[8] | McRae K, Spivey-Knowlton MJ, Tanenhaus MK (1998) Modeling the Influence of Thematic Fit (and Other Constraints) in On-line Sentence Comprehension. J Mem Lang 38: 283–312.
|
[9] | Jurafsky D (1996) A probabilistic model of lexical and syntactic access and disambiguation. Cogn Sci 20: 137–194.
|
[10] | Crocker M, Brants T (2000) Wide-coverage probabilistic sentence processing. J Psycholinguist Res 29: 647–669.
|
[11] | Levy R (2008) Expectation-based syntactic comprehension. Cognition 106: 1126–1177.
|
[12] | Morgan JL, Meier RP, Newport EL (1987) Structural packaging in the input to language learning: Contributions of prosodic and morphological marking of phrases to the acquisition of language. Cogn Psychol 19: 498–550.
|
[13] | Saffran JR, Senghas A, Trueswell JC (2001) The acquisition of language by children. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98: 12874–12875.
|
[14] | Bates E, MacWhinney B (1989) Functionalism and the Competition Model. In: MacWhinney B, Bates E, editors. The Cross-linguistic Study of Sentence Processing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
|
[15] | Trueswell J, Sekerina I, Hill N, Logrip M (1999) The kindergarten-path effect: Studying on-line sentence processing in young children. Cognition 73.
|
[16] | Snedeker J, Trueswell JC (2004) The developing constraints on parsing decisions: The role of lexical-biases and referential scenes in child and adult sentence processing. Cogn Psychol 49: 238–299.
|
[17] | Snedeker J, Thorpe K, Trueswell JC (2001) On choosing the parse with the scene: The role of visual context and verb bias in ambiguity resolution. In: Moore J, Stenning K, editors. Proceedings of the 23rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale: Erlbaum. 964–969.
|
[18] | Sekerina IA, Stromswold K, Hestvik A (2004) How do adults and children process referentially ambiguous pronouns? J Child Lang 31: 123–152.
|
[19] | Hurewitz F, Brown-Schmidt S, Thorpe K, Gleitman LR, Trueswell JC (2000) One Frog, Two Frog, Red Frog, Blue Frog: Factors Affecting Children’s Syntactic Choices in Production and Comprehension. J Psycholinguist Res 29: 597–626.
|
[20] | Arnold JE, Brown-Schmidt S, Trueswell J (2007) Children’s use of gender and order-of-mention during pronoun comprehension. Lang Cogn Process 22: 527–565.
|
[21] | Pickering MJ, van Gompel RPG (2006) Syntactic parsing. In: Traxler MJ, Gernsbacher MA, editors. The handbook of psycholinguistics. San Diego: Elsevier. 455–503.
|
[22] | Frazier L (1978) On comprehending sentences: Syntactic parsing strategies. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Connecticut, Storrs.
|
[23] | MacDonald MC (1994) Probabilistic constraints and syntactic ambiguity resolution. Lang Cogn Process 9: 157–201.
|
[24] | Trueswell JC, Tanenhaus MK (1994) Toward a lexicalist framework for constraint-based syntactic ambiguity resolution. In: Clifton C, Frazier L, Rayner K, editors. Perspectives in Sentence Processing. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 155–179.
|
[25] | Van Gompel RPG, Pickering MJ, Traxler MJ (2001) Reanalysis in Sentence Processing: Evidence against Current Constraint-Based and Two-Stage Models. J Mem Lang 45: 225–258.
|
[26] | Hale J (2001) A probabilistic Earley parser as a psycholinguistic model. Proceedings of NAACL 2: 159–166.
|
[27] | Traxler MJ, Pickering MJ, Clifton C (1998) Adjunct Attachment Is Not a Form of Lexical Ambiguity Resolution. J Mem Lang 39: 558–592.
|
[28] | van Gompel RPG, Pickering MJ, Pearson J, Liversedge SP (2005) Evidence against competition during syntactic ambiguity resolution. J Mem Lang 52: 284–307.
|
[29] | Hale J (2001) A probabilistic Earley parser as a psycholinguistic model. Proceedings of NAACL 2: 159–166.
|
[30] | Cooper RM (1974) The control of eye fixation by the meaning of spoken language: A new methodology for the real-time investigation of speech perception, memory, and language processing. Cogn Psychol 6: 84–107.
|
[31] | Tanenhaus MK, Spivey-Knowlton MJ, Eberhard KM, Sedivy JC (1995) Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science 268: 1632–1634.
|
[32] | Kidd E, Stewart AJ, Serratrice L (2011) Children do not overcome lexical biases where adults do: the role of the referential scene in garden-path recovery. J Child Lang 38: 222–234.
|
[33] | Altmann GTM, van Nice KY, Garnham A, Henstra J (1998) Late closure in context. J Mem Lang 38: 459–484.
|
[34] | Kidd E, Bavin EL (2007) Lexical and referential influences on on-line spoken language comprehension: A comparison of adults and primary-school-age children. First Lang 27: 29–52.
|
[35] | Willows DM, Ryan EB (1986) The Development of Grammatical Sensitivity and Its Relationship to Early Reading Achievement. Read Res Q 21: 253–266.
|
[36] | Bowey JA (1986) Syntactic awareness and verbal performance from preschool to fifth grade. J Psycholinguist Res 15: 285–308.
|
[37] | Willows DM, Ryan EB (1981) Differential utilization of syntactic and semantic information by skilled and less skilled readers in the intermediate grades. J Educ Psychol 73: 607–615.
|
[38] | Rayner K (1998) Eye Movements in Reading and Information Processing: 20 Years of Research. Psychol Bull 124: 372–422.
|
[39] | Liversedge SP, Findlay JM (2000) Saccadic eye movements and cognition. Trends Cogn Sci 4: 6–14.
|
[40] | Blythe HI, Joseph HSSL (2011) Children’s eye movements during reading. In: Liversedge SP, Gilchrist ID, Everling S, editors. The Oxford Handbook of Eye Movements. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 643–662.
|
[41] | Blythe HI, H?iki? T, Bertam R, Liversedge SP, Hy?n? J (2011) Reading disappearing text: Why do children refixate words? Vision Res 51: 84–92.
|
[42] | Blythe HI, Liversedge SP, Joseph HSSL, White SJ, Findlay JM, et al. (2006) The binocular co-ordination of eye movements during reading in children and adults. Vision Res 46: 3898–3908.
|
[43] | Blythe HI, Liversedge SP, Joseph HSSL, White SJ, Rayner K (2009) The uptake of visual information during fixations in reading in children and adults. Vision Res 49: 1583–1591.
|
[44] | Huestegge L, Radach R, Corbic D, Huestegge SM (2009) Oculomotor and linguistic determinants of reading development: A longitudinal study. Vision Res 49: 2948–2959.
|
[45] | Joseph HSSL, Liversedge SP, Blythe HI, White SJ, Rayner K (2009) Word length and landing position effects during reading in children and adults. Vision Res 49: 2078–2086.
|
[46] | McConkie GW, Zola D, Grimes J, Kerr PW, Bryant NR, et al.. (1991) Children’s eye movements during reading. In: Stein JF, editor. Vision and visual dyslexia. Boston: CRC Press. 251–262.
|
[47] | Rayner K (1986) Eye movements and the perceptual span in beginning and skilled readers. J Exp Child Psychol 41: 211–236.
|
[48] | Taylor SE (1965) Eye movements while reading: Facts and fallacies. Am Educ Res J 2: 187–202.
|
[49] | H?iki? T, Bertram R, Hy?n? J, Niemi P (2009) Development of the letter identity span in reading: Evidence from the eye movement moving window paradigm. J Exp Child Psychol 102: 167–181.
|
[50] | Hy?n? J, Olson RK (1995) Eye fixation patterns among dyslexic and normal readers: effects of word-length and word-frequency. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 21: 1430–1440.
|
[51] | Joseph HSSL, Liversedge SP, Blythe HI, White SJ, Gathercole SE, et al. (2008) Children’s and adults’ processing of anomaly and implausibility during reading: Evidence from eye movements. Q J Exp Psychol 61: 708–723.
|
[52] | Rust J, Golombok S, Trickey G (1993) Wechsler Objective Reading Dimensions Test. Sidcup, UK: Psychological Corporation.
|
[53] | Spivey-Knowlton M, Sedivy JC (1995) Resolving attachment ambiguities with multiple constraints. Cognition 55: 227–267.
|
[54] | Altmann GTM, Steedman M (1988) Interaction with context during human sentence processing. Cognition 30: 191–238.
|
[55] | Kidd E (2012) Implicit statistical learning is directly associated with the acquisition of syntax. Dev Psychol 48: 171–184.
|
[56] | Tomasello M (2000) The item-based nature of children’s early syntactic development. Trends Cogn Sci 4: 156–163.
|
[57] | Zang C, Liang F, Bai X, Yan G, Liversedge SP (2012) Interword spacing and landing position effects during Chinese reading in children and adults. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform: (in press).
|
[58] | Parviainen T, Helenius P, Poskiparta E, Niemi P, Salmelin R (2006) Cortical sequence of word perception in beginning readers. J Neurosci 26: 6052–6061.
|
[59] | Pollatsek A, Reichle ED, Rayner K (2006) Tests of the E-Z Reader model: Exploring the interface between cognition and eye-movement control. Cogn Psychol 52: 1–56.
|
[60] | Reichle ED, Rayner K, Pollatsek A (2003) The E-Z Reader model of eye-movement control in reading: Comparisons to other models. Behav Brain Sci 26: 445–476.
|
[61] | Engbert R, Longtin A, Kliegl R (2002) A dynamical model of saccade generation in reading based on spatially distributed lexical processing. Vision Res 42: 621–636.
|
[62] | Engbert R, Nuthmann A, Richter E, Kliegl R (2005) SWIFT: A dynamical model of saccade generation during reading. Psychol Rev 112: 777–813.
|
[63] | Yang SN, McConkie GW (2001) Eye movements during reading: a theory of saccade initiation times. Vision Res 41: 3567–3585.
|
[64] | O’Regan JK, Lévy-Schoen A (1987) Eye movement strategy and tactics in word recognition and reading. In: Coltheart M, editor. Attention and performance XII: The psychology of reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 363–383.
|
[65] | Reichle ED, Laurent PA (2006) Using Reinforcement Learning to Understand the Emergence of “Intelligent” Eye-Movement Behavior During Reading. Psychol Rev 113: 390–408.
|
[66] | Liu Y, Reichle ED. (2010) The emergence of adaptive eye movements in reading. In: Ohlsson S, Catrabone R, editors. Austin, Texas. Cognitive Science Society. 1136–1141.
|
[67] | Reichle ED, Liversedge SP, Drieghe D, Blythe HI, Joseph HSSL, et al.. (2012) Using E-Z Reader to Examine the Concurrent Development of Eye-Movement Control and Reading Skill. Dev Rev: (under review).
|
[68] | O’Regan J (1992) Optimal viewing position in words and the strategy-tactics theory of eye movements in reading. In: Rayner K, editor. Eye movements and visual cognition: Scene perception and reading. New York: Springer. 333–354.
|
[69] | O’Regan JK (1990) Eye movements and reading. In: Kowler E, editor. Eye movements and their role in visual and cognitive processes. New York: Elsvier. 395–447.
|
[70] | Yang S, McConkie G (2004) Saccade generation during reading: Are words necessary? Eur J Cogn Psychol 16: 226–261.
|
[71] | Weighall AR (2008) The kindergarten path effect revisited: Children’s use of context in processing structural ambiguities. J Exp Child Psychol 99: 75–95.
|
[72] | Davidson MC, Amso D, Anderson LC, Diamond A (2006) Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia 44: 2037–2078.
|
[73] | Anderson V, Anderson AP, Northam E, Jacobs R, Catroppa C (2001) Development of executive functions through late childhood and adolescence in an Australian sample. Dev Neuropsychol 20: 385–406.
|
[74] | Novick JM, Trueswell JC, Thompson-Schill SL (2005) Cognitive control and parsing: Reexamining the role of Broca’s area in sentence comprehension. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 5: 263–281.
|
[75] | Choi Y, Son H, Lee H, Trueswell J (2011) Re-examining the role of cognitive control in children’s sentence processing. Poster presented at Architectures and Mechanisms of Language Processing Conference, Paris, 2011.
|
[76] | Gathercole SE, Pickering SJ, Ambridge B, Wearing H (2004) The Structure of Working Memory From 4 to 15 Years of Age. Dev Psychol 40: 177–190.
|
[77] | Palladino P, Cornoldi C, De Beni R, Pazzaglia F (2001) Working memory and updating processes in reading comprehension. Mem Cognit 29: 344–354.
|
[78] | Mitchell DC, Shen X, Green MJ, Hodgson TL (2008) Accounting for regressive eye-movements in models of sentence processing: A reappraisal of the Selective Reanalysis hypothesis. J Mem Lang 59: 266–293.
|
[79] | Inhoff AW, Weger UW (2005) Memory for word location during reading: Eye movements to previously read words are spatially selective but not precise. Mem Cognit 33: 447–461.
|
[80] | Weger UW, Inhoff AW (2007) Long-range regressions to previously read words are guided by spatial and verbal memory. Mem Cognit 35: 1293–1306.
|
[81] | Hyona J, Lorch RF Jr, Kaakinen JK (2002) Individual differences in reading to summarize expository text: Evidence from eye fixation patterns. J Educ Psychol 94: 44–55.
|
[82] | Kennedy A, Murray WS (1987) Spatial coordinates and reading-Comments on Monk. Q J Exp Psychol 39: 649–656.
|