Background The evidence for culture in non-human animals has been growing incrementally over the past two decades. However, the ability for cumulative cultural evolution, with successive generations building on earlier achievements, in non-human animals remains debated. Faithful social learning of incremental improvements in technique is considered to be a defining feature of human culture, differentiating human from non-human cultures. This study presents the first experimental evidence for chimpanzees' social transmission of a more efficient tool-use technique invented by a conspecific group member. Methodology/Principal Findings The chimpanzees were provided with a straw-tube, and spontaneously demonstrated two different techniques in obtaining juice through a small hole: “dipping” and “straw-sucking”. Both the “dipping” and “straw-sucking” techniques depended on the use of the same tool (straw-tube) for the same target (juice) accessible from exactly the same location (small hole 1 cm in diameter). Therefore the difference between “dipping” and “straw-sucking” was only in “technique”. Although the two techniques differed significantly in their efficiency, their cognitive and perceptuo-motor complexity were comparable. All five chimpanzees who initially performed the “dipping” technique switched to using the more efficient “straw-sucking” technique upon observing a conspecific or human demonstrate the more proficient alternate “straw-sucking” technique. Conclusions/Significance The social learning mechanism involved here was clearly not local or stimulus enhancement, but imitation or emulation of a tool-use technique. When there is no biologically relevant difference in cognitive or perceptuo-motor complexity between two techniques, and when chimpanzees are dissatisfied with their own technique, chimpanzees may socially learn an improved technique upon close observation of a proficient demonstrator. This study provides important insights into the cognitive basis for cumulative culture in chimpanzees, and also suggests possible conditions in which cumulative cultural evolution could arise even in non-human animals.
References
[1]
Whiten A, Goodall J, McGrew WC, Nishida T, Reynolds V, et al. (1999) Cultures in Chimpanzees. Nature 399: 682–685.
[2]
van Schaik CP, Ancrenaz M, Borgen G, Galdikas B, Knott CD, et al. (2003) Orangutan cultures and the evolution of material culture. Science 299: 102–105.
[3]
McGrew WC (2004) The cultured chimpanzee: Reflections on cultural primatology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
[4]
Boesch C, Tomasello M (1998) Chimpanzee and human cultures. Curr Anthropol 39: 591–614.
[5]
Boyd R, Richerson P (1996) Why culture is common and cultural evolution is rare. In: Runciman W, Maynard Smith J, Dunbar R, editors. Evolution of social behaviour patterns in primates and man. London: British Academy. pp 77–94.
[6]
Mesoudi A, Whiten A, Laland KN (2006) Towards a unified science of cultural evolution. Behav Brain Sci 29: 329–383.
[7]
Tomasello M, Kruger AC, Ratner HH (1993) Cultural learning. Behav Brain Sci 16: 495–511.
[8]
Tomasello M, Call J (1997) Primate Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
[9]
Tennie C, Call J, Tomasello M (2009) Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Phil Trans R Soc B 364: 2405–2415.
[10]
Boesch C (2003) Is culture a golden barrier between human and chimpanzee? Evol Anthropol 12: 82–91.
[11]
Humle T, Matsuzawa T (2002) Ant-dipping among the chimpanzees of Bossou, Guinea, and some comparisons with other sites. Am J Primatol 58: 133–148.
[12]
Matsuzawa T, Yamakoshi G (1996) Comparison of chimpanzee material culture between Bossou and Nimba, West Africa. In: Russon A, Bard K, Parkers S, editors. Reaching into thought. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp 211–232.
[13]
McGrew WC (1974) Tool use by wild chimpanzees in feeding upon driver ants. J Hum Evol 3: 501–508.
[14]
Boesch C, Boesch H (1990) Tool use and tool making in wild chimpanzees. Folia Primatol 54: 86–99.
[15]
Yamakoshi G, Myowa-Yamakoshi M (2004) New observation of ant-dipping techniques in wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea. Primates 45: 25–32.
[16]
M?bius Y, Boesch C, Koops K, Matsuzawa T, Humle T (2008) Cultural differences in army ant predation by West African chimpanzees? A comparative study of microecological variables. Anim Behav 76: 37–45.
[17]
Sch?ning C, Humle T, M?bius Y, McGrew WC (2008) The nature of culture: technological variation in chimpanzee predation on army ants. J Hum Evol 55: 48–59.
[18]
Sanz CM, Schoning C, Morgan DB (2010) Chimpanzees prey on army ants with specialized tool set. Am J Primatol 72: 17–24.
[19]
Mercader J, Barton H, Gillespie J, Harris J, Kuhn S, et al. (2007) 4,300-Year-old chimpanzee sites and the origins of percussive stone technology. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 104: 3043–3048.
[20]
Matsuzawa T (2011) Stone tools for nut-cracking. In: Matsuzawa T, Humle T, Sugiyama Y, editors. The chimpanzees in Bossou and Nimba. Tokyo: Springer. pp. 73–83.
[21]
Matsuzawa T (2001) Primate foundations of human intelligence: a view of tool use in nonhuman primates and fossil hominids. In: Matsuzawa T, editor. Primate origins of human cognition and behavior. Tokyo: Springer. pp 3–25.
[22]
Yamamoto S Invention and modification of new tool-use behavior. In: Carayannis EG, editor, Encyclopedia of creativity, invention, innovation, and entrepreneurship, New York/Heidelberg: Springer. In press.
[23]
Biro D, Inoue-Nakamura N, Tonooka R, Yamakoshi G, Sousa C, et al. (2003) Cultural innovation and transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees: evidence from field experiments. Anim Cogn 6: 213–223.
[24]
Hirata S, Morimura N (2000) Na?ve chimpanzees' (Pan troglodytes) observation of experienced conspecifics in a tool-using task. J Comp Psychol 114: 291–296.
[25]
Matsuzawa T, Biro D, Humle T, Inoue-Nakamura N, Tonooka R, et al.. (2001) Emergence of culture in wild chimpanzees: education by master-apprenticeship. In: Matsuzawa T, editor. Primate origins of human cognition and behavior. Tokyo: Springer-Verlag. pp. 557–574.
[26]
Horner V, Whiten A (2005) Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Anim Cogn 8: 164–181.
[27]
Horner V, Whiten A, Flynn E, de Waal FBM (2006) Faithful replication of foraging techniques along cultural transmission chains by chimpanzees and children. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103: 13878–13883.
[28]
Whiten A, Custance DM, Gomez J, Teixidor P, Bard KA (1996) Imitative learning of artificial fruit processing in children (Homo sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 110: 3–14.
[29]
Whiten A, Horner V, de Waal FBM (2005) Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees. Nature 437: 737–740.
[30]
Marshall-Pescini A, Whiten A (2008) Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and the question of cumulative culture: an experimental approach. Anim Cogn 11: 449–456.
[31]
Hrubesch C, Preuschoft S, van Schaik CP (2009) Skill mastery inhibits adaption of observed alternative solutions among chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Anim Cogn 12: 209–216.
[32]
Whiten A, Ham R (1992) On the nature and evolution of imitation in the animal kingdom: reappraisal of a century of research. In: Slater PJB, Rosenblatt JS, Beer C, Milinski M, editors. On the nature and evolution of imitation in the animal kingdom: reappraisal of a century of research. Academic, New York, pp 239–283.
[33]
Caldwell CA, Millen AE (2009) Social learning mechanisms and cumulative cultural evolution. Is imitation necessary? Psychol Sci 20: 1478–1483.
[34]
Laland KN (2004) Social learning strategies. Learn Behav 32: 4–14.
[35]
Yamamoto S, Yamakoshi G, Humle T, Matsuzawa T (2008) Invention and modification of a new tool use behavior: ant-fishing in trees by a wild chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) at Bossou, Guinea. Am J Primatol 70: 699–702.
[36]
de Waal FBM (2001) The ape and the sushi master: Cultural reflections of a primatologist. New York: Basic Books.
[37]
Horner V, Proctor D, Bonnie KE, Whiten A, de Waal FBM (2010) Prestige affects cultural learning in chimpanzees. PLoS ONE 5 (5) e10625 doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0010625.
[38]
Hirata S, Celli ML (2003) Role of mothers in the acquisition of tool-use behaviours by captive infant chimpanzees. Anim Cong 6: 235–244.
[39]
van Schaik CP, Deaner RO, Merrill MY (1999) The conditions for tool use in primates: implications for the evolution of material culture. J Human Evol 36: 719–741.
[40]
Ochiai T, Matsuzawa T (1998) Planting trees in an outdoor compound of chimpanzees for an enriched environment. Proceedings of the Third International Conference on Environmental Enrichment. Orlando, Fla., 355–364.