[1] | Merleau-Ponty M (1964) The primacy of perception and other essays on phenomenological psychology. Evanston, IL: Nortwestern University Press.
|
[2] | Wittgenstein L (1980) Remarks on the Philosophy of Psychology, vol. 2, In von Wright GH, and Nyman H, editors, Luckhardt CG, and Aue MAE (trans.), Oxford: Blackwell.
|
[3] | Gallagher S, Zahavi D (2008) The Phenomenological Mind. An Introduction to Philosophy of Mind and Cognitive Science. New York and London: Routledge.
|
[4] | Teufel C, Fletcher PC, Davis G (2010) Seeing other minds: attributed mental states influence perception. Trends Cogn Sci 14: 376–382.
|
[5] | Blakemore SJ, Decety J (2001) From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nat Rev Neurosci 2: 561–567.
|
[6] | Niedenthal PM, Barsalou LW, Ric F, Krauth-Gruber S (2005) Embodiment in the acquisition and use of emotion knowledge. In Feldman-Barrett P, Winkielman L, & Niedenthal PM, editors, Emotion: Conscious and unconscious. New York: Guilford Press. 21–50.
|
[7] | Botvinick M, Jha AP, Bylsma LM, Fabian SA, Solomon PE, et al. (2005) Viewing facial expressions of pain engages cortical areas involved in the direct experience of pain. Neuroimage 25: 312–319.
|
[8] | Carr L, Iacoboni M, Dubeau MC, Mazziotta JC, Lenzi GL (2003) Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: a relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100: 5497–5502.
|
[9] | Leslie KR, Johnson-Frey SH, Grafton ST (2004) Functional imaging of face and hand imitation: towards a motor theory of empathy. Neuroimage 21: 601–607.
|
[10] | Wicker B, Keysers C, Plailly J, Royet JP, Gallese V, et al. (2003) Both of us disgusted in My insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron 40: 655–664.
|
[11] | Gallese V, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Rizzolatti G (1996) Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain 119: 593–609.
|
[12] | Rizzolatti G, Craighero L (2004) The mirror-neuron system. Annu Rev Neurosci 27: 169–192.
|
[13] | Niedenthal PM, Mermillod M, Maringer M, Hess U (2010) The Simulation of Smiles (SIMS) Model: Embodied simulation and the meaning of facial expression, Brain and Behav Sci. 33: 417–480.
|
[14] | Cattaneo L, Fabbri-Destro M, Boria S, Pieraccini C, Monti A, et al. (2007) Impairment of actions chains in autism and its possible role in intention understanding. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104: 17825–17830.
|
[15] | Hudson M, Liu CH, Jellema T (2009) Anticipating intentional actions: The effect of eye gaze direction on the judgement of head rotation. Cognition 112: 423–434.
|
[16] | Hudson M, Jellema T (2011) Resolving ambiguous behavioural intentions by means of involuntary prioritisation of gaze processing. Emotion 11: 681–686.
|
[17] | Teufel C, Alexis DM, Clayton NS, Davis G (2010) Mental-state attribution drives rapid, reflexive gaze following. Atten Percept Psychophys 72: 695–705.
|
[18] | Jellema T, Pecchinenda A, Palumbo L, Tan EG (2011) Biases in the perception and affective valence of neutral facial expressions induced by the immediate perceptual history. Vis Cogn 19: 616–634.
|
[19] | Sato W, Yoshikawa S (2004) The dynamic aspects of emotional facial expressions. Cogn Emot 18: 701–710.
|
[20] | Suzuki S, Cavanagh P (1998) A shape-contrast effect for briefly presented stimuli. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept and Perform 24: 1315–1341.
|
[21] | Thayer S (1980) The effect of expression sequence and expressor identity on judgments of intensity of facial expression. J Nonverbal Behav 5: 71–79.
|
[22] | Russell JA (1980) A circumplex model of affect. J Pers Soc Psychol 39: 1161–1178.
|
[23] | Ekman P, O’Sullivan M (1988) The role of context in interpreting facial expressions – Comment. J Exp Psychol Gen 117: 86–88.
|
[24] | Tanaka-Matsumi J, Attivissimo D, Nelson S, D’urso T (1995) Context effects on the judgment of basic emotions in the face. Motiv Emot 19: 139–155.
|
[25] | Finke RA, Freyd JJ (1985) Transformation of visual memory induced by implied motions of pattern elements. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 11: 780–794.
|
[26] | Freyd JJ, Finke RA (1984) Representational Momentum. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 10: 126–132.
|
[27] | Hubbard TL (2005) Representational momentum and related displacements in spatial memory: A review of the findings. Psychon Bull Rev 12: 822–851.
|
[28] | Freyd JJ (1983) The mental representation of movement when static stimuli are viewed. Percept Psychophys 33: 575–581.
|
[29] | Vinson NG, Reed CL (2002) Sources of object-specific effects in representational momentum. Vis Cogn 9: 41–65.
|
[30] | Kerzel D (2000) Eye movements and visible persistence explain the mislocalisation of the final position of a moving target. Vision Res 40: 3703–3715.
|
[31] | Yoshikawa S, Sato W (2008) Dynamic expressions of emotion induce representational momentum. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 8: 25–31.
|
[32] | Hsu SM (2004) Young AW (2004) Adaptation effects in facial expression recognition. Vis Cogn 11: 871–899.
|
[33] | Webster MA, Kaping D, Mizokami Y, Duhamel P (2004) Adaptation to natural facial categories. Nature 428: 557–561.
|
[34] | Fox CJ, Barton JJ (2007) What is adapted in face adaptation? The neural representations of expression in the human visual system. Brain Res 1127: 80–89.
|
[35] | Ellamil M, Susskind JM, Anderson AK (2008) Examinations of identity invariance in facial expressions adaptation. Cogn Affect & Behav Neurosci 8: 273–281.
|
[36] | Leopold DA, O’Toole AJ, Vetter T, Blanz V (2001) Prototype-referenced shape encoding revealed by high-level aftereffects. Nat Neurosci 4: 89–94.
|
[37] | Kovacs G, Cziraki C, Vidnyanszky Z, Schweinberger SR, Greenlee MW (2008) Position-specific and position-invariant face aftereffects reflect the adaptation of different cortical areas. NeuroImage 43: 156–164.
|
[38] | Mühlberger A, Wieser MJ, Gerdes ABM, Frey MCM, Weyers P, et al. (2011) Stop looking angry and smile, please: start and stop of the very same facial expression differentially activate threat- and reward-related brain networks. Soc Cogn and Affect Neurosci 6: 321–329.
|
[39] | Goldman AI (2006) Simulating Minds: The Philosophy, Psychology and Neuroscience of Mindreading. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
|
[40] | Baron-Cohen S (1995) Mindblindness: an essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
|
[41] | Weiner B, Handel S (1986) A cognition-emotion-action sequence: anticipated emotional consequences of causal attribution and reported communication strategy. Dev Psychol 21: 102–107.
|
[42] | Rao R, Ballard D (1999) Predictive coding in the visual cortex: A functional Interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nat Neurosci 2: 79.
|
[43] | Friston K (2010) The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? Nat Rev Neurosci 11: 127–138.
|
[44] | Clark A Whatever Next? Predictive Brains, Situated Agents, and the Future of Cognitive Science. Behav Brain Sci: In press.
|
[45] | Ekman P, Friesen WV (1976) Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
|
[46] | Perrett DI, May KA, Yoshikawa S (1994) Facial shape and judgements of female actractiveness. Nature 368: 239–242.
|
[47] | Palermo R, Rhodes G (2007) Are you always on my mind? A review of how face perception and attention interact. Neuropsychologia 45: 75–92.
|
[48] | Davidson RJ, Hughdahl K (Eds.) (1995) Brain asymmetry. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
|
[49] | Hess U, Adams RB (2007) Kleck RE (2007) Looking at you or looking elsewhere: The influence of head orientation on the signal value of emotional facial expressions. Motiv Emot 31: 137–144.
|
[50] | Calvo MG, Marrero H (2009) Visual search of emotional faces: The role of affective content and featural distinctiveness. Cogn Emot 23: 782–806.
|
[51] | Neta M, Whalen PJ (2010) The Primacy of Negative Interpretations When Resolving the Valence of Ambiguous Facial Expressions. Psychol Sci 21: 901–907.
|
[52] | Enns JT, Lleras A, Moore MC (2010) Object updating: A force for perceptual continuity and scene stability in human vision. In: Nijhawan R, editor. Problems of space and time in perception and action. Cambridge University Press. 503–520.
|
[53] | Jones BC, DeBruine LM, Little AC, Conway CA, Feinburg DR (2006) Integrating gaze direction and expression in preferences for attractive faces. Psychol Sci 17: 588–591.
|
[54] | Ekman P, Friesen WV (1975) Unmasking the face. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
|
[55] | Leslie AM (1994) Pretending and believing - issues in the theory of ToMM. Cognition 50: 211–238.
|
[56] | Gordon RM (2004) Folk psychology as mental simulation. In: Zalta N, editor. The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy.
|
[57] | Gallese V (2007) Before and below ‘theory of mind’: Embodied simulation and the neural correlates of social cognition. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 362: 659–669.
|
[58] | Gallese V, Goldman A (1998) Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends Cogn Sci 2: 493–501.
|
[59] | Jellema T, Perrett DI (2002) Coding of visible and hidden objects. Attention and Performance 19: 356–380.
|
[60] | Jellema T, Perrett DI (2005) Neural basis for the perception of goal-directed actions. In: Easton A and Emery NJ, editors. The Cognitive Neuroscience of Social Behaviour. New York: Psychology Press. 81–112.
|
[61] | Allison T, Puce A, McCarthy G (2000) Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends Cogn Sci 4: 267–278.
|
[62] | Haxby JV, Hoffman EA, Gobbini MI (2002) Human neural systems for face recognition and social communication. Biol Psychiatry 51: 59–67.
|
[63] | Jellema T, Perrett DI (2003) Perceptual history influences neural responses to face and body postures. J Cogn Neurosci 15: 961–971.
|
[64] | Perrett DI, Xiao D, Barraclough NE, Keysers C, Oram MW (2009) Seeing the future: natural image sequences produce ‘anticipatory’ neuronal activity and bias perceptual report. Q J Exp Psychol 62: 2081–2104.
|
[65] | Jellema T, Baker CI, Wicker B, Perrett DI (2000) Neural representation for the perception of the intentionality of actions. Brain Cogn 44: 280–302.
|
[66] | Saxe R, Xiao DK, Kovacs G, Perrett DI, Kanwisher N (2004) A region of right posterior superior temporal sulcus responds to observed intentional actions. Neuropsychologia 42: 1435–1446.
|
[67] | Saxe R (2006) Uniquely human social cognition. Curr Opin Neurobiol 16: 235–239.
|
[68] | Rozzi S, Calzavara R, Belmalih A, Borra E, Gregoriou GG, et al. (2006) Cortical connections of the inferior parietal cortical convexity of the macaque monkey. Cereb Cortex 16: 1389–1417.
|
[69] | Dapretto M, Davies MS, Pfeifer JH, Scott AA, Sigman M, et al. (2006) Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nat Neurosci 9: 28–30.
|
[70] | Niedenthal PM (2007) Embodying emotion. Science 316: 1002–1005.
|
[71] | Pitcher D, Garrido L, Walsh V, Duchaine BC (2008) Transcranial magnetic stimulation disrupts the perception and embodiment of facial expressions. J Neurosc 28: 8929–8933.
|
[72] | Likowski KU, Mühlberger A, Gerdes ABM, Wieser MJ, Pauli P, et al. (2012) Facial mimicry and the mirror neuron system: simultaneous acquisition of facial electromyography and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6: 214.
|
[73] | Ekman P (2003) Emotions revealed: Recognizing faces and feelings to improve communication and emotional life. New York: Times Books/Henry Holt.
|
[74] | Dimberg U, Thunberg M, Elmehed K (2000) Unconscious facial reactions to emotional facial expressions. Psychol Sci 11: 86–89.
|
[75] | Fodor JA (1968) The appeal to tacit knowledge in psychological explanation. J Phil 65: 627–640.
|
[76] | Stich S, Nichols S (1992) Folk psychology: Simulation or tacit theory? Mind Lang 7: 35–71.
|
[77] | Goldman AI, Sripada CS (2005) Simulationist models of face-based emotion recognition. Cognition 94: 193–213.
|