[1] | Ydenberg R, Dill L (1986) The economics of fleeing from predators. Ad Study Behav 16: 229–249.
|
[2] | Cooper WE Jr, Frederick WG (2007) Optimal flight initiation distance. J Theor Biol 244: 59–67.
|
[3] | Stankowich T, Blumstein DT (2005) Fear in animals: a meta-analysis and review of risk assessment. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 272: 2627–2634.
|
[4] | Lima SL, Dill LM (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 68: 619–640.
|
[5] | Helfman GS (1989) Threat-sensitive predator avoidance in damselfish-trumpetfish interactions. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 24: 47–58.
|
[6] | Cooper WE Jr (1997) Factors affecting risk and cost of escape by the broad-headed skink (Eumeces laticeps): predator speed, directness of approach, and female presence. Herpetologica 53: 464–474.
|
[7] | Stankowich T, Coss RG (2006) Effects of predator behavior and proximity on risk assessment by Columbian black-tailed deer. Behav Ecol 17: 246–254.
|
[8] | Bateman P, Fleming P (2011) Who are you looking at? Hadeda ibises use direction of gaze, head orientation and approach speed in their risk assessment of a potential predator. J Zool 285: 316–323.
|
[9] | Cooper WE Jr, López P, Martín J, Pérez-Mellado V (2012) Latency to flee from an immobile predator: effects of predation risk and cost of immobility for the prey. Behav Ecol 23: 790–797.
|
[10] | Helfman GS, Winkelman DL (1997) Threat sensitivity in bicolor damselfish: effects of sociality and body size. Ethology 103: 369–383.
|
[11] | Blumstein DT (2010) Flush early and avoid the rush: a general rule of antipredator behavior? Behav Ecol 21: 440–442.
|
[12] | Blumstein DT (2003) Flight-initiation distance in birds is dependent on intruder starting distance. J Wildl Manage 67: 852–857.
|
[13] | Endler J (1986) Defense against predators. In Feder EM, Lauder GV editors. Predator-prey relationships: perspectives and approaches from the study of lower vertebrates. Chicago:University of Chicago Press.pp. 169–202.
|
[14] | Broom M, Ruxton GD (2005) You can run—or you can hide: optimal strategies for cryptic prey against pursuit predators. Behav Ecol 16: 534–540.
|
[15] | Cooper WE Jr (2005) When and how do predator starting distances affect flight initiation distances? Can J Zool 83: 1045–1050.
|
[16] | Griffin DR, Webster FA, Michael CR (1960) The echolocation of flying insects by bats. Anim Behav 8: 141–154.
|
[17] | Holderied MW, Von Helversen O (2003) Echolocation range and wingbeat period match in aerial-hawking bats. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 270: 2293–2299.
|
[18] | Moss CF, Surlykke A (2001) Auditory scene analysis by echolocation in bats. J Acoust Soc Am 110: 2207–2226.
|
[19] | Ghose K, Moss CF (2003) The sonar beam pattern of a flying bat as it tracks tethered insects. J Acoust Soc Am 114: 1120–1131.
|
[20] | Kick SA, Simmons JA (1984) Automatic gain control in the bat's sonar receiver and the neuroethology of echolocation. J Neurosci 4: 2725–2737.
|
[21] | Hartley DJ (1992) Stabilization of perceived echo amplitudes in echolocating bats. I. Echo detection and automatic gain control in the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus, and the fishing bat, Noctilio leporinus. J Acoust Soc Am 91: 1120–1132.
|
[22] | Hartley DJ (1992) Stabilization of perceived echo amplitudes in echolocating bats. II. The acoustic behavior of the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus, when tracking moving prey. J Acoust Soc Am 91: 1133–1149.
|
[23] | Boonman A, Jones G (2002) Intensity control during target approach in echolocating bats; stereotypical sensori-motor behaviour in Daubenton's bats, Myotis daubentonii. J Exp Biol 205: 2865–2874.
|
[24] | Hiryu S, Hagino T, Riquimaroux H, Watanabe Y (2007) Echo-intensity compensation in echolocating bats (Pipistrellus abramus) during flight measured by a telemetry microphone. J Acoust Soc Am 121: 1749–1757.
|
[25] | Elemans CPH, Mead AF, Jakobsen L, Ratcliffe JM (2011) Superfast muscles set maximum call rate in echolocating bats. Science 333: 1885–1888.
|
[26] | Hoy RR (1998) Acute as a bug's ear: an informal discussion of hearing in insects. In: Hoy RR, Popper AN and Fay RR, editors. Comparative hearing: insects. New York:Springerpp.1– 17.
|
[27] | Conner WE, Corcoran AJ (2012) Sound Strategies: The 65 million-year-old battle between bats and insects. Annu Rev Entomol 57: 21–39.
|
[28] | Roeder KD (1967) Nerve cells and insect behavior. CambridgeMA : Harvard Univ. Pres. 229 p.
|
[29] | Goerlitz HR, Ter Hofstede HM, Zeale MRK, Jones G, Holderied MW (2010) An aerial-hawking bat uses stealth echolocation to counter moth hearing. Curr Biol 20: 1568–1572.
|
[30] | Hristov NI, Conner WE (2005) Sound strategy: acoustic aposematism in the bat–tiger moth arms race. Naturwissenschaften 92: 164–169.
|
[31] | Barber JR, Conner WE (2007) Acoustic mimicry in a predator–prey interaction. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104: 9331.
|
[32] | Corcoran AJ, Barber JR, Conner WE (2009) Tiger moth jams bat sonar. Science 325: 325–327.
|
[33] | Corcoran AJ, Barber JR, Hristov NI, Conner WE (2011) How do tiger moths jam bat sonar? J Exp Biol 214: 2416–2425.
|
[34] | Corcoran AJ, Conner WE (2012) Sonar jamming in the field: effectiveness and behavior of a unique prey defense. J Exp Biol 215: 4278–4287.
|
[35] | Barber JR, Conner WE (2006) Tiger moth responses to a simulated bat attack: timing and duty cycle. J Exp Biol 209: 2637–2650.
|
[36] | Corcoran AJ, Conner WE, Barber JR (2010) Anti-bat tiger moth sounds: Form and function. Curr Zool 56: 358–369.
|
[37] | Fullard JH, Ratcliffe JM, Christie CG (2007) Acoustic feature recognition in the dogbane tiger moth, Cycnia tenera. J Exp Biol 210: 2481–2488.
|
[38] | Ratcliffe JM, Fullard JH, Arthur BJ, Hoy RR (2009) Tiger moths and the threat of bats: decision-making based on the activity of a single sensory neuron. Biol Lett 5: 368–371.
|
[39] | Fullard JH (1979) Behavioral analyses of auditory sensitivity in Cycnia tenera Hübner (Lepidoptera: Arctiidae). J Comp Physiol A 129: 79–83.
|
[40] | Fullard JH (1984) Listening for bats: pulse repetition rate as a cue for a defensive behavior in Cycnia tenera (Lepidoptera: Arctiidae). J Comp Physiol A 154: 249–252.
|
[41] | Fullard JH, Dawson JW, Jacobs DS (2003) Auditory encoding during the last moment of a moth's life. J Exp Biol 206: 281–294.
|
[42] | Fullard JH, Simmons JA, Saillant PA (1994) Jamming bat echolocation: the dogbane tiger moth Cycnia tenera times its clicks to the terminal attack calls of the big brown bat Eptesicus fuscus. J Exp Biol 194: 285–298.
|
[43] | Ratcliffe JM, Fullard JH (2005) The adaptive function of tiger moth clicks against echolocating bats: an experimental and synthetic approach. J Exp Biol 208: 4689–4698.
|
[44] | Ratcliffe JM, Nydam ML (2008) Multimodal warning signals for a multiple predator world. Nature 455: 96–99.
|
[45] | Corcoran AJ (2007) Automated acoustic identification of nine bat species of the eastern United States. Master's thesis ,Humboldt State University.
|
[46] | Svoboda T, Martinec D, Pajdla T (2005) A convenient multicamera self-calibration for virtual environments. Presence 14: 407–422.
|
[47] | Barber JR, Chadwell BA, Garrett N, Schmidt-French B, Conner WE (2009) Na?ve bats discriminate arctiid moth warning sounds but generalize their aposematic meaning. J Exp Biol 212: 2141–2148.
|
[48] | Stapells DR, Picton TW, Smith AD (1982) Normal hearing threshold for clicks. J Acoust Soc Am 72: 74–79.
|
[49] | Harley DJ, Suthers RA (1989) The sound emission pattern of the echolocating bat, Eptesicus fuscus. J Acoust Soc Am 85 1348: 1351.
|
[50] | Bazley E (1976) Sound absorption in air at frequencies up to 100 kHz. National Physics Laboratory Acoustics Report no. AC74.
|
[51] | Jakobsen L, Surlykke A (2010) Vespertilionid bats control the width of their biosonar sound beam dynamically during prey pursuit. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107: 13930–13935.
|
[52] | Sokal RR, Rohlf FJ (1995) The principles and practice of statistics in biological research. New York: Freeman and Company. 887 p.
|
[53] | Zuur AF, Ieno EN, Walker NJ, Saveliev AA, Smith GM (2009) Mixed effects models and extensions in ecology with R. New York: Springer.574p.
|
[54] | R Development Core Team (2010) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
|
[55] | Pinheiro J, Bates D, DebRoy S, Sarkar D (2009) R Core Team (2009) nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. R package version 3 1–96.
|
[56] | Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS (2007) Using multivariate statistics. Boston:Pearson . 1008 p.
|
[57] | Bradley AP (1997) The use of the area under the ROC curve in the evaluation of machine learning algorithms. Pattern Recognit 30: 1145–1159.
|
[58] | Blumstein DT, Fernandex-Juricic E, Zollner PA, Garity SC (2005) Inter-specific variation in avian responses to human disturbance. J Appl Ecol 42: 943–953.
|
[59] | Cooper WE Jr (2009) Optimal escape theory predicts escape behaviors beyond flight initiation distance: risk assessment and escape by striped plateau lizards Sceloporus virgatus. Curr Zool 55: 123–131.
|
[60] | Cooper WE Jr (1997) Threat factors affecting antipredatory behavior in the broad-headed skink (Eumeces laticeps): repeated approach, change in predator path, and predator's field of view. Copeia 1997: 613–619.
|
[61] | Cooper WE Jr (1998) Direction of predator turning, a neglected cue to predation risk. Behaviour 135: 55–64.
|
[62] | Cooper WE Jr (2006) Dynamic risk assessment: prey rapidly adjust flight initiation distance to changes in predator approach speed. Ethology 112: 858–864.
|
[63] | Caro T (2005) Antipredator defenses in birds and mammals. Chicago:University of Chicago Press . 592 p.
|