全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...
PLOS ONE  2013 

Motherese in Interaction: At the Cross-Road of Emotion and Cognition? (A Systematic Review)

DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0078103

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Various aspects of motherese also known as infant-directed speech (IDS) have been studied for many years. As it is a widespread phenomenon, it is suspected to play some important roles in infant development. Therefore, our purpose was to provide an update of the evidence accumulated by reviewing all of the empirical or experimental studies that have been published since 1966 on IDS driving factors and impacts. Two databases were screened and 144 relevant studies were retained. General linguistic and prosodic characteristics of IDS were found in a variety of languages, and IDS was not restricted to mothers. IDS varied with factors associated with the caregiver (e.g., cultural, psychological and physiological) and the infant (e.g., reactivity and interactive feedback). IDS promoted infants’ affect, attention and language learning. Cognitive aspects of IDS have been widely studied whereas affective ones still need to be developed. However, during interactions, the following two observations were notable: (1) IDS prosody reflects emotional charges and meets infants’ preferences, and (2) mother-infant contingency and synchrony are crucial for IDS production and prolongation. Thus, IDS is part of an interactive loop that may play an important role in infants’ cognitive and social development.

References

[1]  Saxton M (2008) What's in a name? Coming to terms with the child's linguistic environment. J Child Lang 35: 677-686. PubMed: 18588720.
[2]  Ferguson CA (1964) Baby Talk in Six Languages. Am Anthropol 66: 103-114. doi:10.1525/aa.1964.66.suppl_3.02a00060.
[3]  Soderstrom M, Morgan JL (2007) Twenty-two-month-olds discriminate fluent from disfluent adult-directed speech. Dev Sci 10: 641-653. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00605.x. PubMed: 17683348.
[4]  Snow CE, Ferguson CA (1977) Talking to children. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
[5]  Fisher C, Tokura H (1995) The given-new contract in speech to infants. J Mem Lang 34: 287-310. doi:10.1006/jmla.1995.1013.
[6]  Grieser DL, Kuhl PK (1988) Maternal speech to infants in a tonal language: Support for universal prosodic features in motherese. Dev Psychol 24: 14-20. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.24.1.14.
[7]  Soderstrom M, Blossom M, Foygel R, Morgan JL (2008) Acoustical cues and grammatical units in speech to two preverbal infants. J Child Lang 35: 869-902. doi:10.1017/S0305000908008763. PubMed: 18838016.
[8]  Durkin K, Rutter DR, Tucker H (1982) Social interaction and language acquisition: Motherese help you. First Lang 3: 107-120. doi:10.1177/014272378200300803.
[9]  Fernald A, Simon T (1984) Expanded intonation contours in mothers' speech to newborns. Dev Psychol 20: 104-113. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.20.1.104.
[10]  Ogle SA, Maidment JA (1993) Laryngographic analysis of child-directed speech. Eur J Disord Commun 28: 289-297. doi:10.1111/j.1460-6984.1993.tb01570.x. PubMed: 8241583.
[11]  Fernald A, Taeschner T, Dunn J, Papousek M, de Boysson-Bardies B et al. (1989) A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers' and fathers' speech to preverbal infants. J Child Lang 16: 477-501. doi:10.1017/S0305000900010679. PubMed: 2808569.
[12]  Niwano K, Sugai K (2003) Pitch Characteristics of Speech During Mother-Infant and Father-Infant Vocal Interactions. Jpn J Spec Educ 40: 663-674.
[13]  Shute B, Wheldall K (1999) Fundamental frequency and temporal modifications in the speech of British fathers to their children. Educ Psychol 19: 221-233. doi:10.1080/0144341990190208.
[14]  Shute B, Wheldall K (2001) How do grandmothers speak to their grandchildren? Fundamental frequency and temporal modifications in the speech of British grandmothers to their grandchildren. Educ Psychol 21: 493-503. doi:10.1080/01443410120090858.
[15]  Nwokah EE (1987) Maidese versus motherese--is the language input of child and adult caregivers similar? Lang Speech 30 ( 3): 213-237. PubMed: 3503947.
[16]  Fernald A (1989) Intonation and communicative intent in mothers' speech to infants: is the melody the message? Child Dev 60: 1497-1510. doi:10.2307/1130938. PubMed: 2612255.
[17]  Bryant GA, Barrett HC (2007) Recognizing intentions in infant-directed speech: evidence for universals. Psychol Sci 18: 746-751. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01970.x. PubMed: 17680948.
[18]  Katz GS, Cohn JF, Moore CA (1996) A combination of vocal fo dynamic and summary features discriminates between three pragmatic categories of infant-directed speech. Child Dev 67: 205-217. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01729.x. PubMed: 8605829.
[19]  Stern DN, Spieker S, MacKain K (1982) Intonation contours as signals in maternal speech to prelinguistic infants. Dev Psychol 18: 727-735. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.18.5.727.
[20]  Papou?ek M, Papou?ek H, Symmes D (1991) The meanings of melodies in motherese in tone and stress languages. Infant Behav Dev 14: 415-440. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(91)90031-M.
[21]  Slaney M, McRoberts G (2003) BabyEars: A recognition system for affective vocalizations. Speech Commun 39: 367-384. doi:10.1016/S0167-6393(02)00049-3.
[22]  Trainor LJ, Austin CM, Desjardins RN (2000) Is infant-directed speech prosody a result of the vocal expression of emotion? Psychol Sci 11: 188-195. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00240. PubMed: 11273402.
[23]  Nwokah EE, Hsu HC, Davies P, Fogel A (1999) The integration of laughter and speech in vocal communication: a dynamic systems perspective. J Speech Lang Hear Res 42: 880-894. PubMed: 10450908.
[24]  Burnham D, Kitamura C, Vollmer-Conna U (2002) What's new, pussycat? On talking to babies and animals. Science 296: 1435-1435. doi:10.1126/science.1069587. PubMed: 12029126.
[25]  Swanson LA, Leonard LB, Gandour J (1992) Vowel duration in mothers' speech to young children. J Speech Hear Res 35: 617-625. PubMed: 1608253.
[26]  Swanson LA, Leonard LB (1994) Duration of function-word vowels in mothers' speech to young children. J Speech Hear Res 37: 1394-1405. PubMed: 7877296.
[27]  Fernald A, Mazzie C (1991) Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults. Dev Psychol 27: 209-221. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.209.
[28]  Albin DD, Echols CH (1996) Stressed and word-final syllables in infant-directed speech. Infant Behav Dev 19: 401-418. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(96)90002-8.
[29]  Inoue T, Nakagawa R, Kondou M, Koga T, Shinohara K (2011) Discrimination between mothers' infant- and adult-directed speech using hidden Markov models. Neurosci Res, 70: 62–70. PubMed: 21256898.
[30]  Mahdhaoui A, Chetouani M, Cassel RS, Saint-Georges C, Parlato E, et al. (2011) Computerized home video detection for motherese may help to study impaired interaction between infants who become autistic and their parents. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 20: e6-e18. doi:10.1002/mpr.332. PubMed: 21574205.
[31]  Cristià A (2010) Phonetic enhancement of sibilants in infant-directed speech. J Acoust Soc Am 128: 424-434. doi:10.1121/1.3436529. PubMed: 20649236.
[32]  Englund K, Behne D (2006) Changes in Infant Directed Speech in the First Six Months. Infant Child Dev 15: 139-160. doi:10.1002/icd.445.
[33]  Englund KT, Behne DM (2005) Infant directed speech in natural interaction--Norwegian vowel quantity and quality. J Psycholinguist Res 34: 259-280. doi:10.1007/s10936-005-3640-7. PubMed: 16050445.
[34]  Englund K (2005) Voice onset time in infant directed speech over the first six months. First Lang 25: 219-234. doi:10.1177/0142723705050286.
[35]  Green JR, Nip IS, Wilson EM, Mefferd AS, Yunusova Y (2010) Lip movement exaggerations during infant-directed speech. J Speech Lang Hear Res 53: 1529-1542. doi:10.1044/1092-4388 (2010/09-0005). PubMed : 20699342.
[36]  Lee SA, Davis B, Macneilage P (2010) Universal production patterns and ambient language influences in babbling: a cross-linguistic study of Korean- and English-learning infants. J Child Lang England, 37: 293-318. PubMed: 19570317.
[37]  Shute B, Wheldall K (1989) Pitch alterations in British motherese: some preliminary acoustic data. J Child Lang 16: 503-512. doi:10.1017/S0305000900010680. PubMed: 2808570.
[38]  Segal O, Nir-Sagiv B, Kishon-Rabin L, Ravid D (2009) Prosodic patterns in Hebrew child-directed speech. J Child Lang 36: 629-656. doi:10.1017/S030500090800915X. PubMed: 19006600.
[39]  Lee S, Davis BL, Macneilage PF (2008) Segmental properties of input to infants: a study of Korean. J Child Lang 35: 591-617. PubMed: 18588716.
[40]  Grieser DL, Kuhl K (1988) Maternal speech to infants in a tonal language: Support for universal prosodic features in motherese. Dev Psychol 24: 14-20. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.24.1.14.
[41]  Liu HM, Tsao FM, Kuhl PK (2007) Acoustic analysis of lexical tone in Mandarin infant-directed speech. Dev Psychol 43: 912-917. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.912. PubMed: 17605524.
[42]  Fais L, Kajikawa S, Amano S, Werker JF (2010) Now you hear it, now you don't: vowel devoicing in Japanese infant-directed speech. J Child Lang England, 37: 319-340. PubMed: 19490747.
[43]  Masataka N (1992) Motherese in a signed language. Infant Behav Dev 15: 453-460. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(92)80013-K.
[44]  Masataka N (1998) Perception of motherese in Japanese sign language by 6-month-old hearing infants. Dev Psychol 34: 241-246. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.34.2.241. PubMed: 9541776.
[45]  Reilly JS, Bellugi U (1996) Competition on the face: affect and language in ASL motherese. J Child Lang 23: 219-239. PubMed: 8733568.
[46]  Werker JF, Pons F, Dietrich C, Kajikawa S, Fais L et al. (2007) Infant-directed speech supports phonetic category learning in English and Japanese. Cognition 103: 147-162. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.03.006. PubMed: 16707119.
[47]  Kitamura C, Burnham D (2003) Pitch and communicative intent in mother's speech: Adjustments for age and sex in the first year. Infancy 4: 85-110. doi:10.1207/S15327078IN0401_5.
[48]  Stern DN, Spieker S, Barnett RK, MacKain K (1983) The prosody of maternal speech: infant age and context related changes. J Child Lang 10: 1-15. PubMed: 6841483.
[49]  Liu HM, Tsao FM, Kuhl PK (2009) Age-related changes in acoustic modifications of Mandarin maternal speech to preverbal infants and five-year-old children: a longitudinal study. J Child Lang 36: 909-922. doi:10.1017/S030500090800929X. PubMed: 19232142.
[50]  Niwano K, Sugai K (2002) Intonation contour of Japanese maternal infant-directed speech and infant vocal response. Jpn J Spec Educ 39: 59-68.
[51]  Kajikawa S, Amano S, Kondo T (2004) Speech overlap in Japanese mother-child conversations. J Child Lang 31: 215-230. doi:10.1017/S0305000903005968. PubMed: 15053091.
[52]  Amano S, Nakatani T, Kondo T (2006) Fundamental frequency of infants' and parents' utterances in longitudinal recordings. J Acoust Soc Am 119: 1636-1647. doi:10.1121/1.2161443. PubMed: 16583908.
[53]  Snow (1972) Mother's speech to children learning language. Child Dev 43: 549-565. doi:10.2307/1127555.
[54]  Kitamura C, Thanavishuth C, Burnham D, Luksaneeyanawin S (2002) Universality and specificity in infant-directed speech: Pitch modifications as a function of infant age and sex in a tonal and non-tonal language. Infant Behav Dev 24: 372-392.
[55]  Feldman R (2007) Parent–infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 48: 329-354. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01701.x. PubMed: 17355401.
[56]  Gleason JB (1977) Talking to children: some notes on feed-back. In: F. Sa. Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
[57]  Braarud HC, Stormark KM (2008) Prosodic modification and vocal adjustments in mothers' speech during face-to-face interaction with their two- to four-month-old infants: A double video study. Soc Dev 17: 1074-1084. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00455.x.
[58]  Smith NA, Trainor LJ (2008) Infant-directed speech is modulated by infant feedback. Infancy 13: 410-420. doi:10.1080/15250000802188719.
[59]  Shimura Y, Yamanoucho I (1992) Sound spectrographic studies on the relation between motherese and pleasure vocalization in early infancy. Acta Paediatr Jpn 34: 259-266. doi:10.1111/j.1442-200X.1992.tb00956.x. PubMed: 1509871.
[60]  Van Puyvelde M, Vanfleteren P, Loots G, Deschuyffeleer S, Vinck B et al. (2010) Tonal synchrony in mother-infant interaction based on harmonic and pentatonic series. Infant Behav Dev 33: 387-400. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.04.003. PubMed: 20478620.
[61]  McRoberts GW, Best CT (1997) Accommodation in mean f0 during mother-infant and father-infant vocal interactions: a longitudinal case study. J Child Lang 24: 719-736. doi:10.1017/S030500099700322X. PubMed: 9519592.
[62]  Reissland N, Stephenson T (1999) Turn-taking in early vocal interaction: a comparison of premature and term infants' vocal interaction with their mothers. Child Care Health Dev 25: 447-456. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2214.1999.00109.x. PubMed: 10547707.
[63]  Niwano K, Sugai K (2003) Maternal accommodation in infant-directed speech during mother's and twin-infants' vocal interactions. Psychol Rep 92: 481-487. doi:10.2466/pr0.2003.92.2.481. PubMed: 12785629.
[64]  Reissland N, Shepherd J, Cowie L (2002) The melody of surprise: maternal surprise vocalizations during play with her infant. Infant Child Dev 11: 271-278. doi:10.1002/icd.258.
[65]  Lederberg AR (1984) Interaction between deaf preschoolers and unfamiliar hearing adults. Child Dev 55: 598-606. doi:10.2307/1129971. PubMed: 6723449.
[66]  Fidler DJ (2003) Parental vocalizations and perceived immaturity in down syndrome. Am J Ment Retard 108: 425-434. doi:10.1352/0895-8017(2003)108. PubMed: 14561106.
[67]  Gogate LJ, Bahrick LE, Watson JD (2000) A study of multimodal motherese: The role of temporal synchrony between verbal labels and gestures. Child Dev 71: 878-894. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00197. PubMed: 11016554.
[68]  Kavanaugh RD, Jirkovsky AM (1982) Parental speech to young children: A longitudinal analysis. Merrill-Palmer. Q: J of Dev Psych 28: 297-311.
[69]  Bohannon JN, Marquis AL (1977) Children's control of adult speech. Child Dev 48: 1002-1008. doi:10.2307/1128352.
[70]  Bergeson TR, Miller RJ, McCune K (2006) Mothers' Speech to Hearing-Impaired Infants and Children With Cochlear Implants. Infancy 10: 221-240. doi:10.1207/s15327078in1003_2.
[71]  Kondaurova MV, Bergeson TR (2010) The effects of age and infant hearing status on maternal use of prosodic cues for clause boundaries in speech. J Speech. Lang Hear Res.
[72]  Ikeda Y, Masataka N (1999) A variable that may affect individual differences in the child-directed speech of Japanese women. Jpn Psychol Res 41: 203-208. doi:10.1111/1468-5884.00120.
[73]  Hoff E, Tian C (2005) Socioeconomic status and cultural influences on language. J Commun Disord 38: 271-278. doi:10.1016/j.jcomdis.2005.02.003. PubMed: 15862810.
[74]  Hoff-Ginsberg E (1991) Mother-child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Dev 62: 782-796. doi:10.2307/1131177. PubMed: 1935343.
[75]  Rowe ML (2008) Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. J Child Lang 35: 185-205. PubMed: 18300434.
[76]  Matsuda YT, Ueno K, Waggoner RA, Erickson D, Shimura Y et al. (2011) Processing of infant-directed speech by adults. NeuroImage 54: 611-621. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.072. PubMed: 20691794.
[77]  Gordon I, Zagoory-Sharon O, Leckman JF, Feldman R (2010) Oxytocin and the development of parenting in humans. Biol Psychiatry 68: 377-382. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.02.005. PubMed: 20359699.
[78]  Bettes BA (1988) Maternal depression and motherese: temporal and intonational features. Child Dev 59: 1089-1096. doi:10.2307/1130275. PubMed: 3168616.
[79]  Herrera E, Reissland N, Shepherd J (2004) Maternal touch and maternal child-directed speech: effects of depressed mood in the postnatal period. J Affect Disord 81: 29-39. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2003.07.001. PubMed: 15183597.
[80]  Kaplan PS, bachorowski J-A, Smoski MJ, Zinser M (2001) Role of clinical diagnosis and medication use in effects of maternal depression on infant-directed speech. Infancy 2: 537-548. doi:10.1207/S15327078IN0204_08.
[81]  Wan MW, Penketh V, Salmon MP, Abel KM (2008) Content and style of speech from mothers with schizophrenia towards their infants. Psychiatry Res 159: 109-114. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2007.05.012. PubMed: 18329722.
[82]  McLeod PJ (1993) What studies of communication with infants ask us about psychology: Baby-talk and other speech registers. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie canadienne 34: 282-292.
[83]  Fernald A (1993) Approval and disapproval: Infant responsiveness to vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. Child Dev 64: 657-674. doi:10.2307/1131209. PubMed: 8339687.
[84]  Papou?ek M, Bornstein MH, Nuzzo C, Papou?ek H (1990) Infant responses to prototypical melodic contours in parental speech. Infant Behav Dev 13: 539-545. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(90)90022-Z.
[85]  Elena S, Merideth G (2012) Infants infer intentions from prosody. Cogn Dev 27: 1-16. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2011.08.003.
[86]  Santesso DL, Schmidt LA, Trainor LJ (2007) Frontal brain electrical activity (EEG) and heart rate in response to affective infant-directed (ID) speech in 9-month-old infants. Brain Cogn 65: 14-21. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2007.02.008. PubMed: 17659820.
[87]  Monnot M (1999) Function of infant-directed speech. Hum Nat 10: 415-443. doi:10.1007/s12110-999-1010-0.
[88]  Fernald A, Kuhl P (1987) Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech. Infant Behav Dev 10: 279-293. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(87)90017-8.
[89]  Santarcangelo S, Dyer K (1988) Prosodic aspects of motherese: effects on gaze and responsiveness in developmentally disabled children. J Exp Child Psychol 46: 406-418. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(88)90069-0. PubMed: 3216186.
[90]  Schachner A, Hannon EE (2010) Infant-directed speech drives social preferences in 5-month-old infants. Dev Psychol, 47: 19–25. PubMed: 20873920.
[91]  Werker JF, McLeod PJ (1989) Infant preference for both male and female infant-directed talk: a developmental study of attentional and affective responsiveness. Can J Psychol 43: 230-246. doi:10.1037/h0084224. PubMed: 2486497.
[92]  Cooper RP (1993) The effect of prosody on young infants' speech perception. Advances Infancy Res 8: 137-167.
[93]  Cooper RP, Aslin RN (1990) Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Dev 61: 1584-1595. doi:10.2307/1130766. PubMed: 2245748.
[94]  Pegg JE, Werker JF, McLeod PJ (1992) Preference for infant-directed over adult-directed speech: Evidence from 7-week-old infants. Infant Behav Dev 15: 325-345. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(92)80003-D.
[95]  Saito Y, Aoyama S, Kondo T, Fukumoto R, Konishi N et al. (2007) Frontal cerebral blood flow change associated with infant-directed speech. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed England, 92: F113-F116. PubMed: 16905571.
[96]  Hayashi A, Tamekawa Y, Kiritani S (2001) Developmental change in auditory preferences for speech stimuli in Japanese infants. J Speech Lang Hear Res 44: 1189-1200. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2001/092). PubMed: 11776357.
[97]  Newman RS, Hussain I (2006) Changes in Preference for Infant-Directed Speech in Low and Moderate Noise by 4.5- to 13-Month-Olds. Infancy 10: 61-76. doi:10.1207/s15327078in1001_4.
[98]  Cooper RP, Abraham J, Berman S, Staska M (1997) The development of infants' preference for motherese. Infant Behav Dev 20: 477-488. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(97)90037-0.
[99]  Hepper PG, Scott D, Shahidullah S (1993) Newborn and fetal response to maternal voice. J Reprod Infant Psychol 11: 147-153. doi:10.1080/02646839308403210.
[100]  Panneton R, Kitamura C, Mattock K, Burnham D (2006) Slow Speech Enhances Younger But Not Older Infants' Perception of Vocal Emotion. Res Hum Dev 3: 7-19. doi:10.1207/s15427617rhd0301_2.
[101]  Niwano K, Sugai K (2002) Acoustic determinants eliciting Japanese infants' vocal response to maternal speech. Psychol Rep 90: 83-90. doi:10.2466/pr0.2002.90.1.83. PubMed: 11899017.
[102]  Kitamura C, Lam C (2009) Age-specific preferences for infant-directed affective intent. Infancy 14: 77-100. doi:10.1080/15250000802569777.
[103]  Kaplan PS, Owren MJ (1994) Dishabituation of visual attention in 4-month-olds by infant-directed frequency sweeps. Infant Behav Dev 17: 347-358. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(94)90027-2.
[104]  Spence MJ, Moore DS (2003) Categorization of infant-directed speech: development from 4 to 6 months. Dev Psychobiol 42: 97-109. doi:10.1002/dev.10093. PubMed: 12471640.
[105]  Johnson DM, Dixon DR, Coon RC, Hilker K, Gouvier WD (2002) Watch what you say and how you say it: differential response to speech by participants with and without head injuries. Appl Neuropsychol 9: 58-62. doi:10.1207/S15324826AN0901_7. PubMed: 12173751.
[106]  Cooper RP, Aslin RN (1994) Developmental differences in infant attention to the spectral properties of infant-directed speech. Child Dev 65: 1663-1677. doi:10.2307/1131286. PubMed: 7859548.
[107]  Leibold LJ, Werner LA (2007) Infant auditory sensitivity to pure tones and frequency-modulated tones. Infancy 12: 225-233. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00241.x.
[108]  Trainor LJ, Zacharias CA (1998) Infants prefer higher-pitched singing. Infant Behav Dev 21: 799-805. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(98)90047-9.
[109]  Colombo J, Frick JE, Ryther JS, Coldren JT (1995) Infants' detection of analogs of 'motherese' in noise. Merrill-Palmer. Q: J of Dev Psych 41: 104-113.
[110]  Singh L, Morgan JL, Best CT (2002) Infants' Listening Preferences: Baby Talk or Happy Talk? Infancy 3: 365-394. doi:10.1207/S15327078IN0303_5.
[111]  McRoberts GW, McDonough C, Lakusta L (2009) The role of verbal repetition in the development of infant speech preferences from 4 to 14 months of age. Infancy 14: 162-194. doi:10.1080/15250000802707062.
[112]  Nakata T, Trehub SE (2004) Infants' responsiveness to maternal speech and singing. Infant Behav Dev 27: 455-464. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2004.03.002.
[113]  Trehub SE, Nakata T (2001) Emotion and music in infancy. Musicae Sci Spec Issue: 2001-2002: 37-61.
[114]  Kaplan PS, Goldstein MH, Huckeby ER, Cooper RP (1995) Habituation, sensitization, and infants' responses to motherese speech. Dev Psychobiol 28: 45-57. doi:10.1002/dev.420280105. PubMed: 7895923.
[115]  Kaplan PS, Jung PC, Ryther JS, Zarlengo-Strouse P (1996) Infant-directed versus adult-directed speech as signals for faces. Dev Psychol 32: 880-891. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.32.5.880.
[116]  Senju A, Csibra G (2008) Gaze following in human infants depends on communicative signals. Curr Biol 18: 668-671. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.03.059. PubMed: 18439827.
[117]  Gergely G, Egyed K, Király I (2007) On pedagogy. Dev Sci 10: 139-146. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00576.x. PubMed: 17181712.
[118]  Kaplan PS, Bachorowski JA, Smoski MJ, Hudenko WJ (2002) Infants of depressed mothers, although competent learners, fail to learn in response to their own mothers' infant-directed speech. Psychol Sci 13: 268-271. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00449. PubMed: 12009049.
[119]  Kaplan PS, Bachorowski JA, Zarlengo-Strouse P (1999) Child-directed speech produced by mothers with symptoms of depression fails to promote associative learning in 4-month-old infants. Child Dev 70: 560-570. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00041. PubMed: 10368910.
[120]  Kaplan PS, Danko CM, Diaz A, Kalinka CJ (2010) An associative learning deficit in 1-year-old infants of depressed mothers: Role of depression duration. Infant Behav Dev.
[121]  Kaplan PS, Dungan JK, Zinser MC (2004) Infants of chronically depressed mothers learn in response to male, but not female, infant-directed speech. Dev Psychol 40: 140-148. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.140. PubMed: 14979756.
[122]  Kaplan PS, Danko CM, Diaz A (2010) A Privileged Status for Male Infant-Directed Speech in Infants of Depressed Mothers? Role of Father Involvement. Infancy 15: 151-175. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00010.x.
[123]  Kaplan PS, Sliter JK, Burgess AP (2007) Infant-directed speech produced by fathers with symptoms of depression: effects on infant associative learning in a conditioned-attention paradigm. Infant Behav Dev 30: 535-545. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.05.003. PubMed: 17604106.
[124]  Kaplan PS, Burgess AP, Sliter JK, Moreno AJ (2009) Maternal Sensitivity and the Learning-Promoting Effects of Depressed and Non-Depressed Mothers' Infant-Directed Speech. Infancy 14: 143-161. doi:10.1080/15250000802706924. PubMed: 20046973.
[125]  Karzon RG (1985) Discrimination of polysyllabic sequences by one- to four-month-old infants. J Exp Child Psychol 39: 326-342. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(85)90044-X. PubMed: 3989467.
[126]  Karzon RG, Nicholas JG (1989) Syllabic pitch perception in 2- to 3-month-old infants. Percept Psychophys 45: 10-14. doi:10.3758/BF03208026. PubMed: 2913563.
[127]  Vallabha GK, McClelland JL, Pons F, Werker JF, Amano S (2007) Unsupervised learning of vowel categories from infant-directed speech. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104: 13273-13278. doi:10.1073/pnas.0705369104. PubMed: 17664424.
[128]  Trainor LJ, Desjardins RN (2002) Pitch characteristics of infant-directed speech affect infants' ability to discriminate vowels. Psychon Bull Rev 9: 335-340. doi:10.3758/BF03196290. PubMed: 12120797.
[129]  Hirsh-Pasek K, Kemler Nelson DG, Jusczyk PW, Cassidy KW (1987) Clauses are perceptual units for young infants. Cognition 26: 269-286. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(87)80002-1. PubMed: 3677573.
[130]  Kemler Nelson DG, Hirsh-Pasek K, Jusczyk PW, Cassidy KW (1989) How the prosodic cues in motherese might assist language learning. J Child Lang 16: 55-68. doi:10.1017/S030500090001343X. PubMed: 2925815.
[131]  Thiessen ED, Hill EA, Saffran JR (2005) Infant-Directed Speech Facilitates Word Segmentation. Infancy 7: 53-71. doi:10.1207/s15327078in0701_5.
[132]  D'Odorico L, Jacob V (2006) Prosodic and lexical aspects of maternal linguistic input to late-talking toddlers. Int J Lang Commun Disord 41: 293-311. doi:10.1080/13682820500342976. PubMed: 16702095.
[133]  Morgan JL (1996) Prosody and the roots of parsing. Lang Cogn Processes 11: 69-106. doi:10.1080/016909696387222.
[134]  Curtin S, Mintz TH, Christiansen MH (2005) Stress changes the representational landscape: evidence from word segmentation. Cognition 96: 233-262. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.08.005. PubMed: 15996560.
[135]  Bortfeld H, Morgan JL (2010) Is early word-form processing stress-full? How natural variability supports recognition. Cogn Psychol 60: 241-266. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.01.002. PubMed: 20159653.
[136]  Singh L (2008) Influences of high and low variability on infant word recognition. Cognition 106: 833-870. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.05.002. PubMed: 17586482.
[137]  Kirchhoff K, Schimmel S (2005) Statistical properties of infant-directed versus adult-directed speech: insights from speech recognition. J Acoust Soc Am 117: 2238-2246. doi:10.1121/1.1869172. PubMed: 15898664.
[138]  Singh L, Nestor S, Parikh C, Yull A (2009) Influences of infant-directed speech on early word recognition. Infancy 14: 654-666. doi:10.1080/15250000903263973.
[139]  Song JY, Demuth K, Morgan J (2010) Effects of the acoustic properties of infant-directed speech on infant word recognition. J Acoust Soc Am 128: 389-400. doi:10.1121/1.3419786. PubMed: 20649233.
[140]  Bard EG, Anderson AH (1983) The unintelligibility of speech to children. J Child Lang 10: 265-292. PubMed: 6874768.
[141]  Zangl R, Mills DL (2007) Increased Brain Activity to Infant-Directed Speech in 6-and 13-Month-Old Infants. Infancy 11: 31-62. doi:10.1207/s15327078in1101_2.
[142]  Newport E, Gleitman H, Gleitman L (1977) Mother, I'd rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style.?. In: CSaC, Ferguson. Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
[143]  Furrow D, Nelson K, Benedict H (1979) Mothers' speech to children and syntactic development: Some simple relationships. J Child Lang 6: 423-442. PubMed: 536408.
[144]  Gleitman LR, Newport EL, Gleitman H (1984) The current status of the motherese hypothesis. J Child Lang 11: 43-79. PubMed: 6699113.
[145]  Scarborough H, Wyckoff J (1986) Mother, I'd still rather do it myself: some further non-effects of 'motherese'. J Child Lang 13: 431-437. PubMed: 3745343.
[146]  Furrow D, Nelson K (1986) A further look at the motherese hypothesis: a reply to Gleitman, Newport & Gleitman. J Child Lang 13: 163-176. PubMed: 3949896.
[147]  Hampson J, Nelson K (1993) The relation of maternal language to variation in rate and style of language acquisition. J Child Lang 20: 313-342. PubMed: 8376472.
[148]  Waterfall HR, Sandbank B, Onnis L, Edelman S (2010) An empirical generative framework for computational modeling of language acquisition. J Child Lang 37: 671-703. doi:10.1017/S0305000910000024. PubMed: 20420744.
[149]  Onnis L, Waterfall HR, Edelman S (2008) Learn locally, act globally: learning language from variation set cues. Cognition 109: 423-430. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.10.004. PubMed: 19019350.
[150]  Fernald A, Hurtado N (2006) Names in frames: infants interpret words in sentence frames faster than words in isolation. Dev Sci 9: F33-F40. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00460.x. PubMed: 16669790.
[151]  Golinkoff RM, Alioto A (1995) Infant-directed speech facilitates lexical learning in adults hearing Chinese: implications for language acquisition. J Child Lang 22: 703-726. PubMed: 8789520.
[152]  Kempe V, Brooks PJ, Gillis S (2005) Diminutives in child-directed speech supplement metric with distributional word segmentation cues. Psychon Bull Rev 12: 145-151. doi:10.3758/BF03196360. PubMed: 15945207.
[153]  Kempe V, Brooks PJ, Gillis S, Samson G (2007) Diminutives facilitate word segmentation in natural speech: cross-linguistic evidence. Mem Cogn 35: 762-773. doi:10.3758/BF03193313. PubMed: 17848033.
[154]  Kempe V, Brooks PJ, Mironova N, Fedorova O (2003) Diminutivization supports gender acquisition in Russian children. J Child Lang 30: 471-485. doi:10.1017/S0305000903005580. PubMed: 12846306.
[155]  Seva N, Kempe V, Brooks PJ, Mironova N, Pershukova A et al. (2007) Crosslinguistic evidence for the diminutive advantage: gender agreement in Russian and Serbian children. J Child Lang 34: 111-131. doi:10.1017/S0305000906007720. PubMed: 17340940.
[156]  Hayes DP, Ahrens MG (1988) Vocabulary simplification for children: a special case of 'motherese'? J Child Lang 15: 395-410. doi:10.1017/S0305000900012411. PubMed: 3209647.
[157]  Bunce VL, Harrison DW (1991) Child- or adult-directed speech and esteem: effects on performance and arousal in elderly adults. Int J Aging Hum Dev 32: 125-134. doi:10.2190/JKA7-15D2-0DFT-U934. PubMed: 2055658.
[158]  Hirsh-Pasek K, Treiman R (1982) Doggerel: motherese in a new context. J Child Lang 9: 229-237. PubMed: 7061632.
[159]  Weppelman TL, Bostow A, Schiffer R, Elbert-Perez E, Newman RS (2003) Children's use of the prosodic characteristics of infant-directed speech. Lang Commun 23: 63-80. doi:10.1016/S0271-5309(01)00023-4.
[160]  Rice ML, Haight PL (1986) "Motherese" of Mr. Rogers: a description of the dialogue of educational television programs. J Speech Hear Disord 51: 282-287. PubMed: 3736028.
[161]  Biben M, Symmes D, Bernhards D (1989) Contour variables in vocal communication between squirrel monkey mothers and infants. Dev Psychobiol 22: 617-631. doi:10.1002/dev.420220607. PubMed: 2792572.
[162]  Snowdon CT, Teie D (2010) Affective responses in tamarins elicited by species-specific music. Biol Lett 6: 30-32. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2009.0593. PubMed: 19726444.
[163]  Falk D (2004) Prelinguistic evolution in early hominins: whence motherese? Behav Brain Sci 27: 483-503; discussion: 15773427.
[164]  Fernald A (1992) Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. In: Barkow, Cosmides, Tooby, editors. The Adapted. Mind: 391—428.
[165]  Iverson JM, Capirci O, Longobardi E, Caselli MC (1999) Gesturing in mother–child interactions. Cogn Dev 14: 57-75. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(99)80018-5.
[166]  O'Neill M, Bard KA, Linnell M, Fluck M (2005) Maternal gestures with 20-month-old infants in two contexts. Dev Sci 8: 352-359. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00423.x. PubMed: 15985069.
[167]  Brand RJ, Baldwin DA, Ashburn LA (2002) Evidence for 'motionese': Modifications in mothers' infant-directed action. Dev Sci 5: 72-83. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00211.
[168]  Longhi E (2009) 'Songese': Maternal structuring of musical interaction with infants. Psychol Music 37: 195-213. doi:10.1177/0305735608097042.
[169]  Fernald A (2000) Speech to infants as hyperspeech: knowledge-driven processes in early word recognition. Phonetica 57: 242-254. doi:10.1159/000028477. PubMed: 10992144.
[170]  Matychuk P (2005) The role of child-directed speech in language acquisition: A case study. Lang Sci 27: 301-379. doi:10.1016/j.langsci.2004.04.004.
[171]  Monnot M, Orbelo D, Riccardo L, Sikka S, Rossa E (2003) Acoustic analyses support subjective judgments of vocal emotion. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1000: 288-292. PubMed: 14766639.
[172]  Walker-Andrews AS (1997) Infants' perception of expressive behaviors: differentiation of multimodal information. Psychol Bull 121: 437-456. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.121.3.437. PubMed: 9136644.
[173]  Parncutt R (2009) Prenatal and infant conditioning, the mother schema, and the origins of music and religion. Musicae Sci special issue: 119-150.
[174]  Kuhl PK, Tsao FM, Liu HM (2003) Foreign-language experience in infancy: effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A United States, 100: 9096-9101. PubMed: 12861072.
[175]  Kuhl PK (2007) Is speech learning gated by the social brain? Dev Sci 10: 110-120. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00572.x. PubMed: 17181708.
[176]  Papousek H, Papousek M (1983) Biological basis of social interactions: implications of research for an understanding of behavioural deviance. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 24: 117-129. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1983.tb00109.x. PubMed: 6826670.
[177]  Cohen D, Cassel RS, Saint-Georges C, Mahdhaoui A, Laznik M-C, et al. (2013) Do Parentese Prosody and Fathers' Involvement in Interacting Facilitate Social Interaction in Infants Who Later Develop Autism? PLOS ONE 8: e61402. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0061402. PubMed: 23650498.

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133