[1] | Saxton M (2008) What's in a name? Coming to terms with the child's linguistic environment. J Child Lang 35: 677-686. PubMed: 18588720.
|
[2] | Ferguson CA (1964) Baby Talk in Six Languages. Am Anthropol 66: 103-114. doi:10.1525/aa.1964.66.suppl_3.02a00060.
|
[3] | Soderstrom M, Morgan JL (2007) Twenty-two-month-olds discriminate fluent from disfluent adult-directed speech. Dev Sci 10: 641-653. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00605.x. PubMed: 17683348.
|
[4] | Snow CE, Ferguson CA (1977) Talking to children. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
|
[5] | Fisher C, Tokura H (1995) The given-new contract in speech to infants. J Mem Lang 34: 287-310. doi:10.1006/jmla.1995.1013.
|
[6] | Grieser DL, Kuhl PK (1988) Maternal speech to infants in a tonal language: Support for universal prosodic features in motherese. Dev Psychol 24: 14-20. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.24.1.14.
|
[7] | Soderstrom M, Blossom M, Foygel R, Morgan JL (2008) Acoustical cues and grammatical units in speech to two preverbal infants. J Child Lang 35: 869-902. doi:10.1017/S0305000908008763. PubMed: 18838016.
|
[8] | Durkin K, Rutter DR, Tucker H (1982) Social interaction and language acquisition: Motherese help you. First Lang 3: 107-120. doi:10.1177/014272378200300803.
|
[9] | Fernald A, Simon T (1984) Expanded intonation contours in mothers' speech to newborns. Dev Psychol 20: 104-113. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.20.1.104.
|
[10] | Ogle SA, Maidment JA (1993) Laryngographic analysis of child-directed speech. Eur J Disord Commun 28: 289-297. doi:10.1111/j.1460-6984.1993.tb01570.x. PubMed: 8241583.
|
[11] | Fernald A, Taeschner T, Dunn J, Papousek M, de Boysson-Bardies B et al. (1989) A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers' and fathers' speech to preverbal infants. J Child Lang 16: 477-501. doi:10.1017/S0305000900010679. PubMed: 2808569.
|
[12] | Niwano K, Sugai K (2003) Pitch Characteristics of Speech During Mother-Infant and Father-Infant Vocal Interactions. Jpn J Spec Educ 40: 663-674.
|
[13] | Shute B, Wheldall K (1999) Fundamental frequency and temporal modifications in the speech of British fathers to their children. Educ Psychol 19: 221-233. doi:10.1080/0144341990190208.
|
[14] | Shute B, Wheldall K (2001) How do grandmothers speak to their grandchildren? Fundamental frequency and temporal modifications in the speech of British grandmothers to their grandchildren. Educ Psychol 21: 493-503. doi:10.1080/01443410120090858.
|
[15] | Nwokah EE (1987) Maidese versus motherese--is the language input of child and adult caregivers similar? Lang Speech 30 ( 3): 213-237. PubMed: 3503947.
|
[16] | Fernald A (1989) Intonation and communicative intent in mothers' speech to infants: is the melody the message? Child Dev 60: 1497-1510. doi:10.2307/1130938. PubMed: 2612255.
|
[17] | Bryant GA, Barrett HC (2007) Recognizing intentions in infant-directed speech: evidence for universals. Psychol Sci 18: 746-751. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01970.x. PubMed: 17680948.
|
[18] | Katz GS, Cohn JF, Moore CA (1996) A combination of vocal fo dynamic and summary features discriminates between three pragmatic categories of infant-directed speech. Child Dev 67: 205-217. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01729.x. PubMed: 8605829.
|
[19] | Stern DN, Spieker S, MacKain K (1982) Intonation contours as signals in maternal speech to prelinguistic infants. Dev Psychol 18: 727-735. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.18.5.727.
|
[20] | Papou?ek M, Papou?ek H, Symmes D (1991) The meanings of melodies in motherese in tone and stress languages. Infant Behav Dev 14: 415-440. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(91)90031-M.
|
[21] | Slaney M, McRoberts G (2003) BabyEars: A recognition system for affective vocalizations. Speech Commun 39: 367-384. doi:10.1016/S0167-6393(02)00049-3.
|
[22] | Trainor LJ, Austin CM, Desjardins RN (2000) Is infant-directed speech prosody a result of the vocal expression of emotion? Psychol Sci 11: 188-195. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00240. PubMed: 11273402.
|
[23] | Nwokah EE, Hsu HC, Davies P, Fogel A (1999) The integration of laughter and speech in vocal communication: a dynamic systems perspective. J Speech Lang Hear Res 42: 880-894. PubMed: 10450908.
|
[24] | Burnham D, Kitamura C, Vollmer-Conna U (2002) What's new, pussycat? On talking to babies and animals. Science 296: 1435-1435. doi:10.1126/science.1069587. PubMed: 12029126.
|
[25] | Swanson LA, Leonard LB, Gandour J (1992) Vowel duration in mothers' speech to young children. J Speech Hear Res 35: 617-625. PubMed: 1608253.
|
[26] | Swanson LA, Leonard LB (1994) Duration of function-word vowels in mothers' speech to young children. J Speech Hear Res 37: 1394-1405. PubMed: 7877296.
|
[27] | Fernald A, Mazzie C (1991) Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults. Dev Psychol 27: 209-221. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.209.
|
[28] | Albin DD, Echols CH (1996) Stressed and word-final syllables in infant-directed speech. Infant Behav Dev 19: 401-418. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(96)90002-8.
|
[29] | Inoue T, Nakagawa R, Kondou M, Koga T, Shinohara K (2011) Discrimination between mothers' infant- and adult-directed speech using hidden Markov models. Neurosci Res, 70: 62–70. PubMed: 21256898.
|
[30] | Mahdhaoui A, Chetouani M, Cassel RS, Saint-Georges C, Parlato E, et al. (2011) Computerized home video detection for motherese may help to study impaired interaction between infants who become autistic and their parents. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 20: e6-e18. doi:10.1002/mpr.332. PubMed: 21574205.
|
[31] | Cristià A (2010) Phonetic enhancement of sibilants in infant-directed speech. J Acoust Soc Am 128: 424-434. doi:10.1121/1.3436529. PubMed: 20649236.
|
[32] | Englund K, Behne D (2006) Changes in Infant Directed Speech in the First Six Months. Infant Child Dev 15: 139-160. doi:10.1002/icd.445.
|
[33] | Englund KT, Behne DM (2005) Infant directed speech in natural interaction--Norwegian vowel quantity and quality. J Psycholinguist Res 34: 259-280. doi:10.1007/s10936-005-3640-7. PubMed: 16050445.
|
[34] | Englund K (2005) Voice onset time in infant directed speech over the first six months. First Lang 25: 219-234. doi:10.1177/0142723705050286.
|
[35] | Green JR, Nip IS, Wilson EM, Mefferd AS, Yunusova Y (2010) Lip movement exaggerations during infant-directed speech. J Speech Lang Hear Res 53: 1529-1542. doi:10.1044/1092-4388 (2010/09-0005). PubMed : 20699342.
|
[36] | Lee SA, Davis B, Macneilage P (2010) Universal production patterns and ambient language influences in babbling: a cross-linguistic study of Korean- and English-learning infants. J Child Lang England, 37: 293-318. PubMed: 19570317.
|
[37] | Shute B, Wheldall K (1989) Pitch alterations in British motherese: some preliminary acoustic data. J Child Lang 16: 503-512. doi:10.1017/S0305000900010680. PubMed: 2808570.
|
[38] | Segal O, Nir-Sagiv B, Kishon-Rabin L, Ravid D (2009) Prosodic patterns in Hebrew child-directed speech. J Child Lang 36: 629-656. doi:10.1017/S030500090800915X. PubMed: 19006600.
|
[39] | Lee S, Davis BL, Macneilage PF (2008) Segmental properties of input to infants: a study of Korean. J Child Lang 35: 591-617. PubMed: 18588716.
|
[40] | Grieser DL, Kuhl K (1988) Maternal speech to infants in a tonal language: Support for universal prosodic features in motherese. Dev Psychol 24: 14-20. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.24.1.14.
|
[41] | Liu HM, Tsao FM, Kuhl PK (2007) Acoustic analysis of lexical tone in Mandarin infant-directed speech. Dev Psychol 43: 912-917. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.912. PubMed: 17605524.
|
[42] | Fais L, Kajikawa S, Amano S, Werker JF (2010) Now you hear it, now you don't: vowel devoicing in Japanese infant-directed speech. J Child Lang England, 37: 319-340. PubMed: 19490747.
|
[43] | Masataka N (1992) Motherese in a signed language. Infant Behav Dev 15: 453-460. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(92)80013-K.
|
[44] | Masataka N (1998) Perception of motherese in Japanese sign language by 6-month-old hearing infants. Dev Psychol 34: 241-246. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.34.2.241. PubMed: 9541776.
|
[45] | Reilly JS, Bellugi U (1996) Competition on the face: affect and language in ASL motherese. J Child Lang 23: 219-239. PubMed: 8733568.
|
[46] | Werker JF, Pons F, Dietrich C, Kajikawa S, Fais L et al. (2007) Infant-directed speech supports phonetic category learning in English and Japanese. Cognition 103: 147-162. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.03.006. PubMed: 16707119.
|
[47] | Kitamura C, Burnham D (2003) Pitch and communicative intent in mother's speech: Adjustments for age and sex in the first year. Infancy 4: 85-110. doi:10.1207/S15327078IN0401_5.
|
[48] | Stern DN, Spieker S, Barnett RK, MacKain K (1983) The prosody of maternal speech: infant age and context related changes. J Child Lang 10: 1-15. PubMed: 6841483.
|
[49] | Liu HM, Tsao FM, Kuhl PK (2009) Age-related changes in acoustic modifications of Mandarin maternal speech to preverbal infants and five-year-old children: a longitudinal study. J Child Lang 36: 909-922. doi:10.1017/S030500090800929X. PubMed: 19232142.
|
[50] | Niwano K, Sugai K (2002) Intonation contour of Japanese maternal infant-directed speech and infant vocal response. Jpn J Spec Educ 39: 59-68.
|
[51] | Kajikawa S, Amano S, Kondo T (2004) Speech overlap in Japanese mother-child conversations. J Child Lang 31: 215-230. doi:10.1017/S0305000903005968. PubMed: 15053091.
|
[52] | Amano S, Nakatani T, Kondo T (2006) Fundamental frequency of infants' and parents' utterances in longitudinal recordings. J Acoust Soc Am 119: 1636-1647. doi:10.1121/1.2161443. PubMed: 16583908.
|
[53] | Snow (1972) Mother's speech to children learning language. Child Dev 43: 549-565. doi:10.2307/1127555.
|
[54] | Kitamura C, Thanavishuth C, Burnham D, Luksaneeyanawin S (2002) Universality and specificity in infant-directed speech: Pitch modifications as a function of infant age and sex in a tonal and non-tonal language. Infant Behav Dev 24: 372-392.
|
[55] | Feldman R (2007) Parent–infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 48: 329-354. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01701.x. PubMed: 17355401.
|
[56] | Gleason JB (1977) Talking to children: some notes on feed-back. In: F. Sa. Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
|
[57] | Braarud HC, Stormark KM (2008) Prosodic modification and vocal adjustments in mothers' speech during face-to-face interaction with their two- to four-month-old infants: A double video study. Soc Dev 17: 1074-1084. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00455.x.
|
[58] | Smith NA, Trainor LJ (2008) Infant-directed speech is modulated by infant feedback. Infancy 13: 410-420. doi:10.1080/15250000802188719.
|
[59] | Shimura Y, Yamanoucho I (1992) Sound spectrographic studies on the relation between motherese and pleasure vocalization in early infancy. Acta Paediatr Jpn 34: 259-266. doi:10.1111/j.1442-200X.1992.tb00956.x. PubMed: 1509871.
|
[60] | Van Puyvelde M, Vanfleteren P, Loots G, Deschuyffeleer S, Vinck B et al. (2010) Tonal synchrony in mother-infant interaction based on harmonic and pentatonic series. Infant Behav Dev 33: 387-400. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.04.003. PubMed: 20478620.
|
[61] | McRoberts GW, Best CT (1997) Accommodation in mean f0 during mother-infant and father-infant vocal interactions: a longitudinal case study. J Child Lang 24: 719-736. doi:10.1017/S030500099700322X. PubMed: 9519592.
|
[62] | Reissland N, Stephenson T (1999) Turn-taking in early vocal interaction: a comparison of premature and term infants' vocal interaction with their mothers. Child Care Health Dev 25: 447-456. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2214.1999.00109.x. PubMed: 10547707.
|
[63] | Niwano K, Sugai K (2003) Maternal accommodation in infant-directed speech during mother's and twin-infants' vocal interactions. Psychol Rep 92: 481-487. doi:10.2466/pr0.2003.92.2.481. PubMed: 12785629.
|
[64] | Reissland N, Shepherd J, Cowie L (2002) The melody of surprise: maternal surprise vocalizations during play with her infant. Infant Child Dev 11: 271-278. doi:10.1002/icd.258.
|
[65] | Lederberg AR (1984) Interaction between deaf preschoolers and unfamiliar hearing adults. Child Dev 55: 598-606. doi:10.2307/1129971. PubMed: 6723449.
|
[66] | Fidler DJ (2003) Parental vocalizations and perceived immaturity in down syndrome. Am J Ment Retard 108: 425-434. doi:10.1352/0895-8017(2003)108. PubMed: 14561106.
|
[67] | Gogate LJ, Bahrick LE, Watson JD (2000) A study of multimodal motherese: The role of temporal synchrony between verbal labels and gestures. Child Dev 71: 878-894. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00197. PubMed: 11016554.
|
[68] | Kavanaugh RD, Jirkovsky AM (1982) Parental speech to young children: A longitudinal analysis. Merrill-Palmer. Q: J of Dev Psych 28: 297-311.
|
[69] | Bohannon JN, Marquis AL (1977) Children's control of adult speech. Child Dev 48: 1002-1008. doi:10.2307/1128352.
|
[70] | Bergeson TR, Miller RJ, McCune K (2006) Mothers' Speech to Hearing-Impaired Infants and Children With Cochlear Implants. Infancy 10: 221-240. doi:10.1207/s15327078in1003_2.
|
[71] | Kondaurova MV, Bergeson TR (2010) The effects of age and infant hearing status on maternal use of prosodic cues for clause boundaries in speech. J Speech. Lang Hear Res.
|
[72] | Ikeda Y, Masataka N (1999) A variable that may affect individual differences in the child-directed speech of Japanese women. Jpn Psychol Res 41: 203-208. doi:10.1111/1468-5884.00120.
|
[73] | Hoff E, Tian C (2005) Socioeconomic status and cultural influences on language. J Commun Disord 38: 271-278. doi:10.1016/j.jcomdis.2005.02.003. PubMed: 15862810.
|
[74] | Hoff-Ginsberg E (1991) Mother-child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Dev 62: 782-796. doi:10.2307/1131177. PubMed: 1935343.
|
[75] | Rowe ML (2008) Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. J Child Lang 35: 185-205. PubMed: 18300434.
|
[76] | Matsuda YT, Ueno K, Waggoner RA, Erickson D, Shimura Y et al. (2011) Processing of infant-directed speech by adults. NeuroImage 54: 611-621. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.072. PubMed: 20691794.
|
[77] | Gordon I, Zagoory-Sharon O, Leckman JF, Feldman R (2010) Oxytocin and the development of parenting in humans. Biol Psychiatry 68: 377-382. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.02.005. PubMed: 20359699.
|
[78] | Bettes BA (1988) Maternal depression and motherese: temporal and intonational features. Child Dev 59: 1089-1096. doi:10.2307/1130275. PubMed: 3168616.
|
[79] | Herrera E, Reissland N, Shepherd J (2004) Maternal touch and maternal child-directed speech: effects of depressed mood in the postnatal period. J Affect Disord 81: 29-39. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2003.07.001. PubMed: 15183597.
|
[80] | Kaplan PS, bachorowski J-A, Smoski MJ, Zinser M (2001) Role of clinical diagnosis and medication use in effects of maternal depression on infant-directed speech. Infancy 2: 537-548. doi:10.1207/S15327078IN0204_08.
|
[81] | Wan MW, Penketh V, Salmon MP, Abel KM (2008) Content and style of speech from mothers with schizophrenia towards their infants. Psychiatry Res 159: 109-114. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2007.05.012. PubMed: 18329722.
|
[82] | McLeod PJ (1993) What studies of communication with infants ask us about psychology: Baby-talk and other speech registers. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie canadienne 34: 282-292.
|
[83] | Fernald A (1993) Approval and disapproval: Infant responsiveness to vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. Child Dev 64: 657-674. doi:10.2307/1131209. PubMed: 8339687.
|
[84] | Papou?ek M, Bornstein MH, Nuzzo C, Papou?ek H (1990) Infant responses to prototypical melodic contours in parental speech. Infant Behav Dev 13: 539-545. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(90)90022-Z.
|
[85] | Elena S, Merideth G (2012) Infants infer intentions from prosody. Cogn Dev 27: 1-16. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2011.08.003.
|
[86] | Santesso DL, Schmidt LA, Trainor LJ (2007) Frontal brain electrical activity (EEG) and heart rate in response to affective infant-directed (ID) speech in 9-month-old infants. Brain Cogn 65: 14-21. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2007.02.008. PubMed: 17659820.
|
[87] | Monnot M (1999) Function of infant-directed speech. Hum Nat 10: 415-443. doi:10.1007/s12110-999-1010-0.
|
[88] | Fernald A, Kuhl P (1987) Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech. Infant Behav Dev 10: 279-293. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(87)90017-8.
|
[89] | Santarcangelo S, Dyer K (1988) Prosodic aspects of motherese: effects on gaze and responsiveness in developmentally disabled children. J Exp Child Psychol 46: 406-418. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(88)90069-0. PubMed: 3216186.
|
[90] | Schachner A, Hannon EE (2010) Infant-directed speech drives social preferences in 5-month-old infants. Dev Psychol, 47: 19–25. PubMed: 20873920.
|
[91] | Werker JF, McLeod PJ (1989) Infant preference for both male and female infant-directed talk: a developmental study of attentional and affective responsiveness. Can J Psychol 43: 230-246. doi:10.1037/h0084224. PubMed: 2486497.
|
[92] | Cooper RP (1993) The effect of prosody on young infants' speech perception. Advances Infancy Res 8: 137-167.
|
[93] | Cooper RP, Aslin RN (1990) Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Dev 61: 1584-1595. doi:10.2307/1130766. PubMed: 2245748.
|
[94] | Pegg JE, Werker JF, McLeod PJ (1992) Preference for infant-directed over adult-directed speech: Evidence from 7-week-old infants. Infant Behav Dev 15: 325-345. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(92)80003-D.
|
[95] | Saito Y, Aoyama S, Kondo T, Fukumoto R, Konishi N et al. (2007) Frontal cerebral blood flow change associated with infant-directed speech. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed England, 92: F113-F116. PubMed: 16905571.
|
[96] | Hayashi A, Tamekawa Y, Kiritani S (2001) Developmental change in auditory preferences for speech stimuli in Japanese infants. J Speech Lang Hear Res 44: 1189-1200. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2001/092). PubMed: 11776357.
|
[97] | Newman RS, Hussain I (2006) Changes in Preference for Infant-Directed Speech in Low and Moderate Noise by 4.5- to 13-Month-Olds. Infancy 10: 61-76. doi:10.1207/s15327078in1001_4.
|
[98] | Cooper RP, Abraham J, Berman S, Staska M (1997) The development of infants' preference for motherese. Infant Behav Dev 20: 477-488. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(97)90037-0.
|
[99] | Hepper PG, Scott D, Shahidullah S (1993) Newborn and fetal response to maternal voice. J Reprod Infant Psychol 11: 147-153. doi:10.1080/02646839308403210.
|
[100] | Panneton R, Kitamura C, Mattock K, Burnham D (2006) Slow Speech Enhances Younger But Not Older Infants' Perception of Vocal Emotion. Res Hum Dev 3: 7-19. doi:10.1207/s15427617rhd0301_2.
|
[101] | Niwano K, Sugai K (2002) Acoustic determinants eliciting Japanese infants' vocal response to maternal speech. Psychol Rep 90: 83-90. doi:10.2466/pr0.2002.90.1.83. PubMed: 11899017.
|
[102] | Kitamura C, Lam C (2009) Age-specific preferences for infant-directed affective intent. Infancy 14: 77-100. doi:10.1080/15250000802569777.
|
[103] | Kaplan PS, Owren MJ (1994) Dishabituation of visual attention in 4-month-olds by infant-directed frequency sweeps. Infant Behav Dev 17: 347-358. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(94)90027-2.
|
[104] | Spence MJ, Moore DS (2003) Categorization of infant-directed speech: development from 4 to 6 months. Dev Psychobiol 42: 97-109. doi:10.1002/dev.10093. PubMed: 12471640.
|
[105] | Johnson DM, Dixon DR, Coon RC, Hilker K, Gouvier WD (2002) Watch what you say and how you say it: differential response to speech by participants with and without head injuries. Appl Neuropsychol 9: 58-62. doi:10.1207/S15324826AN0901_7. PubMed: 12173751.
|
[106] | Cooper RP, Aslin RN (1994) Developmental differences in infant attention to the spectral properties of infant-directed speech. Child Dev 65: 1663-1677. doi:10.2307/1131286. PubMed: 7859548.
|
[107] | Leibold LJ, Werner LA (2007) Infant auditory sensitivity to pure tones and frequency-modulated tones. Infancy 12: 225-233. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00241.x.
|
[108] | Trainor LJ, Zacharias CA (1998) Infants prefer higher-pitched singing. Infant Behav Dev 21: 799-805. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(98)90047-9.
|
[109] | Colombo J, Frick JE, Ryther JS, Coldren JT (1995) Infants' detection of analogs of 'motherese' in noise. Merrill-Palmer. Q: J of Dev Psych 41: 104-113.
|
[110] | Singh L, Morgan JL, Best CT (2002) Infants' Listening Preferences: Baby Talk or Happy Talk? Infancy 3: 365-394. doi:10.1207/S15327078IN0303_5.
|
[111] | McRoberts GW, McDonough C, Lakusta L (2009) The role of verbal repetition in the development of infant speech preferences from 4 to 14 months of age. Infancy 14: 162-194. doi:10.1080/15250000802707062.
|
[112] | Nakata T, Trehub SE (2004) Infants' responsiveness to maternal speech and singing. Infant Behav Dev 27: 455-464. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2004.03.002.
|
[113] | Trehub SE, Nakata T (2001) Emotion and music in infancy. Musicae Sci Spec Issue: 2001-2002: 37-61.
|
[114] | Kaplan PS, Goldstein MH, Huckeby ER, Cooper RP (1995) Habituation, sensitization, and infants' responses to motherese speech. Dev Psychobiol 28: 45-57. doi:10.1002/dev.420280105. PubMed: 7895923.
|
[115] | Kaplan PS, Jung PC, Ryther JS, Zarlengo-Strouse P (1996) Infant-directed versus adult-directed speech as signals for faces. Dev Psychol 32: 880-891. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.32.5.880.
|
[116] | Senju A, Csibra G (2008) Gaze following in human infants depends on communicative signals. Curr Biol 18: 668-671. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.03.059. PubMed: 18439827.
|
[117] | Gergely G, Egyed K, Király I (2007) On pedagogy. Dev Sci 10: 139-146. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00576.x. PubMed: 17181712.
|
[118] | Kaplan PS, Bachorowski JA, Smoski MJ, Hudenko WJ (2002) Infants of depressed mothers, although competent learners, fail to learn in response to their own mothers' infant-directed speech. Psychol Sci 13: 268-271. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00449. PubMed: 12009049.
|
[119] | Kaplan PS, Bachorowski JA, Zarlengo-Strouse P (1999) Child-directed speech produced by mothers with symptoms of depression fails to promote associative learning in 4-month-old infants. Child Dev 70: 560-570. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00041. PubMed: 10368910.
|
[120] | Kaplan PS, Danko CM, Diaz A, Kalinka CJ (2010) An associative learning deficit in 1-year-old infants of depressed mothers: Role of depression duration. Infant Behav Dev.
|
[121] | Kaplan PS, Dungan JK, Zinser MC (2004) Infants of chronically depressed mothers learn in response to male, but not female, infant-directed speech. Dev Psychol 40: 140-148. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.140. PubMed: 14979756.
|
[122] | Kaplan PS, Danko CM, Diaz A (2010) A Privileged Status for Male Infant-Directed Speech in Infants of Depressed Mothers? Role of Father Involvement. Infancy 15: 151-175. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00010.x.
|
[123] | Kaplan PS, Sliter JK, Burgess AP (2007) Infant-directed speech produced by fathers with symptoms of depression: effects on infant associative learning in a conditioned-attention paradigm. Infant Behav Dev 30: 535-545. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.05.003. PubMed: 17604106.
|
[124] | Kaplan PS, Burgess AP, Sliter JK, Moreno AJ (2009) Maternal Sensitivity and the Learning-Promoting Effects of Depressed and Non-Depressed Mothers' Infant-Directed Speech. Infancy 14: 143-161. doi:10.1080/15250000802706924. PubMed: 20046973.
|
[125] | Karzon RG (1985) Discrimination of polysyllabic sequences by one- to four-month-old infants. J Exp Child Psychol 39: 326-342. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(85)90044-X. PubMed: 3989467.
|
[126] | Karzon RG, Nicholas JG (1989) Syllabic pitch perception in 2- to 3-month-old infants. Percept Psychophys 45: 10-14. doi:10.3758/BF03208026. PubMed: 2913563.
|
[127] | Vallabha GK, McClelland JL, Pons F, Werker JF, Amano S (2007) Unsupervised learning of vowel categories from infant-directed speech. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104: 13273-13278. doi:10.1073/pnas.0705369104. PubMed: 17664424.
|
[128] | Trainor LJ, Desjardins RN (2002) Pitch characteristics of infant-directed speech affect infants' ability to discriminate vowels. Psychon Bull Rev 9: 335-340. doi:10.3758/BF03196290. PubMed: 12120797.
|
[129] | Hirsh-Pasek K, Kemler Nelson DG, Jusczyk PW, Cassidy KW (1987) Clauses are perceptual units for young infants. Cognition 26: 269-286. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(87)80002-1. PubMed: 3677573.
|
[130] | Kemler Nelson DG, Hirsh-Pasek K, Jusczyk PW, Cassidy KW (1989) How the prosodic cues in motherese might assist language learning. J Child Lang 16: 55-68. doi:10.1017/S030500090001343X. PubMed: 2925815.
|
[131] | Thiessen ED, Hill EA, Saffran JR (2005) Infant-Directed Speech Facilitates Word Segmentation. Infancy 7: 53-71. doi:10.1207/s15327078in0701_5.
|
[132] | D'Odorico L, Jacob V (2006) Prosodic and lexical aspects of maternal linguistic input to late-talking toddlers. Int J Lang Commun Disord 41: 293-311. doi:10.1080/13682820500342976. PubMed: 16702095.
|
[133] | Morgan JL (1996) Prosody and the roots of parsing. Lang Cogn Processes 11: 69-106. doi:10.1080/016909696387222.
|
[134] | Curtin S, Mintz TH, Christiansen MH (2005) Stress changes the representational landscape: evidence from word segmentation. Cognition 96: 233-262. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.08.005. PubMed: 15996560.
|
[135] | Bortfeld H, Morgan JL (2010) Is early word-form processing stress-full? How natural variability supports recognition. Cogn Psychol 60: 241-266. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.01.002. PubMed: 20159653.
|
[136] | Singh L (2008) Influences of high and low variability on infant word recognition. Cognition 106: 833-870. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.05.002. PubMed: 17586482.
|
[137] | Kirchhoff K, Schimmel S (2005) Statistical properties of infant-directed versus adult-directed speech: insights from speech recognition. J Acoust Soc Am 117: 2238-2246. doi:10.1121/1.1869172. PubMed: 15898664.
|
[138] | Singh L, Nestor S, Parikh C, Yull A (2009) Influences of infant-directed speech on early word recognition. Infancy 14: 654-666. doi:10.1080/15250000903263973.
|
[139] | Song JY, Demuth K, Morgan J (2010) Effects of the acoustic properties of infant-directed speech on infant word recognition. J Acoust Soc Am 128: 389-400. doi:10.1121/1.3419786. PubMed: 20649233.
|
[140] | Bard EG, Anderson AH (1983) The unintelligibility of speech to children. J Child Lang 10: 265-292. PubMed: 6874768.
|
[141] | Zangl R, Mills DL (2007) Increased Brain Activity to Infant-Directed Speech in 6-and 13-Month-Old Infants. Infancy 11: 31-62. doi:10.1207/s15327078in1101_2.
|
[142] | Newport E, Gleitman H, Gleitman L (1977) Mother, I'd rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style.?. In: CSaC, Ferguson. Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
|
[143] | Furrow D, Nelson K, Benedict H (1979) Mothers' speech to children and syntactic development: Some simple relationships. J Child Lang 6: 423-442. PubMed: 536408.
|
[144] | Gleitman LR, Newport EL, Gleitman H (1984) The current status of the motherese hypothesis. J Child Lang 11: 43-79. PubMed: 6699113.
|
[145] | Scarborough H, Wyckoff J (1986) Mother, I'd still rather do it myself: some further non-effects of 'motherese'. J Child Lang 13: 431-437. PubMed: 3745343.
|
[146] | Furrow D, Nelson K (1986) A further look at the motherese hypothesis: a reply to Gleitman, Newport & Gleitman. J Child Lang 13: 163-176. PubMed: 3949896.
|
[147] | Hampson J, Nelson K (1993) The relation of maternal language to variation in rate and style of language acquisition. J Child Lang 20: 313-342. PubMed: 8376472.
|
[148] | Waterfall HR, Sandbank B, Onnis L, Edelman S (2010) An empirical generative framework for computational modeling of language acquisition. J Child Lang 37: 671-703. doi:10.1017/S0305000910000024. PubMed: 20420744.
|
[149] | Onnis L, Waterfall HR, Edelman S (2008) Learn locally, act globally: learning language from variation set cues. Cognition 109: 423-430. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.10.004. PubMed: 19019350.
|
[150] | Fernald A, Hurtado N (2006) Names in frames: infants interpret words in sentence frames faster than words in isolation. Dev Sci 9: F33-F40. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00460.x. PubMed: 16669790.
|
[151] | Golinkoff RM, Alioto A (1995) Infant-directed speech facilitates lexical learning in adults hearing Chinese: implications for language acquisition. J Child Lang 22: 703-726. PubMed: 8789520.
|
[152] | Kempe V, Brooks PJ, Gillis S (2005) Diminutives in child-directed speech supplement metric with distributional word segmentation cues. Psychon Bull Rev 12: 145-151. doi:10.3758/BF03196360. PubMed: 15945207.
|
[153] | Kempe V, Brooks PJ, Gillis S, Samson G (2007) Diminutives facilitate word segmentation in natural speech: cross-linguistic evidence. Mem Cogn 35: 762-773. doi:10.3758/BF03193313. PubMed: 17848033.
|
[154] | Kempe V, Brooks PJ, Mironova N, Fedorova O (2003) Diminutivization supports gender acquisition in Russian children. J Child Lang 30: 471-485. doi:10.1017/S0305000903005580. PubMed: 12846306.
|
[155] | Seva N, Kempe V, Brooks PJ, Mironova N, Pershukova A et al. (2007) Crosslinguistic evidence for the diminutive advantage: gender agreement in Russian and Serbian children. J Child Lang 34: 111-131. doi:10.1017/S0305000906007720. PubMed: 17340940.
|
[156] | Hayes DP, Ahrens MG (1988) Vocabulary simplification for children: a special case of 'motherese'? J Child Lang 15: 395-410. doi:10.1017/S0305000900012411. PubMed: 3209647.
|
[157] | Bunce VL, Harrison DW (1991) Child- or adult-directed speech and esteem: effects on performance and arousal in elderly adults. Int J Aging Hum Dev 32: 125-134. doi:10.2190/JKA7-15D2-0DFT-U934. PubMed: 2055658.
|
[158] | Hirsh-Pasek K, Treiman R (1982) Doggerel: motherese in a new context. J Child Lang 9: 229-237. PubMed: 7061632.
|
[159] | Weppelman TL, Bostow A, Schiffer R, Elbert-Perez E, Newman RS (2003) Children's use of the prosodic characteristics of infant-directed speech. Lang Commun 23: 63-80. doi:10.1016/S0271-5309(01)00023-4.
|
[160] | Rice ML, Haight PL (1986) "Motherese" of Mr. Rogers: a description of the dialogue of educational television programs. J Speech Hear Disord 51: 282-287. PubMed: 3736028.
|
[161] | Biben M, Symmes D, Bernhards D (1989) Contour variables in vocal communication between squirrel monkey mothers and infants. Dev Psychobiol 22: 617-631. doi:10.1002/dev.420220607. PubMed: 2792572.
|
[162] | Snowdon CT, Teie D (2010) Affective responses in tamarins elicited by species-specific music. Biol Lett 6: 30-32. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2009.0593. PubMed: 19726444.
|
[163] | Falk D (2004) Prelinguistic evolution in early hominins: whence motherese? Behav Brain Sci 27: 483-503; discussion: 15773427.
|
[164] | Fernald A (1992) Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. In: Barkow, Cosmides, Tooby, editors. The Adapted. Mind: 391—428.
|
[165] | Iverson JM, Capirci O, Longobardi E, Caselli MC (1999) Gesturing in mother–child interactions. Cogn Dev 14: 57-75. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(99)80018-5.
|
[166] | O'Neill M, Bard KA, Linnell M, Fluck M (2005) Maternal gestures with 20-month-old infants in two contexts. Dev Sci 8: 352-359. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00423.x. PubMed: 15985069.
|
[167] | Brand RJ, Baldwin DA, Ashburn LA (2002) Evidence for 'motionese': Modifications in mothers' infant-directed action. Dev Sci 5: 72-83. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00211.
|
[168] | Longhi E (2009) 'Songese': Maternal structuring of musical interaction with infants. Psychol Music 37: 195-213. doi:10.1177/0305735608097042.
|
[169] | Fernald A (2000) Speech to infants as hyperspeech: knowledge-driven processes in early word recognition. Phonetica 57: 242-254. doi:10.1159/000028477. PubMed: 10992144.
|
[170] | Matychuk P (2005) The role of child-directed speech in language acquisition: A case study. Lang Sci 27: 301-379. doi:10.1016/j.langsci.2004.04.004.
|
[171] | Monnot M, Orbelo D, Riccardo L, Sikka S, Rossa E (2003) Acoustic analyses support subjective judgments of vocal emotion. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1000: 288-292. PubMed: 14766639.
|
[172] | Walker-Andrews AS (1997) Infants' perception of expressive behaviors: differentiation of multimodal information. Psychol Bull 121: 437-456. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.121.3.437. PubMed: 9136644.
|
[173] | Parncutt R (2009) Prenatal and infant conditioning, the mother schema, and the origins of music and religion. Musicae Sci special issue: 119-150.
|
[174] | Kuhl PK, Tsao FM, Liu HM (2003) Foreign-language experience in infancy: effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A United States, 100: 9096-9101. PubMed: 12861072.
|
[175] | Kuhl PK (2007) Is speech learning gated by the social brain? Dev Sci 10: 110-120. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00572.x. PubMed: 17181708.
|
[176] | Papousek H, Papousek M (1983) Biological basis of social interactions: implications of research for an understanding of behavioural deviance. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 24: 117-129. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1983.tb00109.x. PubMed: 6826670.
|
[177] | Cohen D, Cassel RS, Saint-Georges C, Mahdhaoui A, Laznik M-C, et al. (2013) Do Parentese Prosody and Fathers' Involvement in Interacting Facilitate Social Interaction in Infants Who Later Develop Autism? PLOS ONE 8: e61402. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0061402. PubMed: 23650498.
|