[1] | Cheng K (1986) A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation. Cognition 23: 149–178.
|
[2] | Cheng K (2005) Reflections on geometry and navigation. Connect Sci 17: 5–21.
|
[3] | Cheng K, Newcombe NS (2005) Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? Squaring theory and evidence. Psychon B Rev 12: 1–23.
|
[4] | Gallistel CR (1990) The Organization of Learning. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.
|
[5] | Cheng K, Gallistel CR (2005) Shape parameters explain data from spatial transformations: comment on Pearce et al. (2004) and Tommasi & Polli (2004). J Exp Psychol Anim B 31: 254–259.
|
[6] | Pearce JM, Good MA, Jones PM, McGregor A (2004) Transfer of spatial behavior between different environments: implications for theories of spatial learning and for the role of the hippocampus in spatial learning. J Exp Psychol Anim B 30: 135–147.
|
[7] | Tommasi L, Polli C (2004) Representation of two geometric features of the environment in the domestic chick (Gallus gallus). Anim Cogn 7: 53–59.
|
[8] | Kelly DM, Spetch ML (2001) Pigeons encode relative geometry. J Exp Psychol Anim B 27: 417–422.
|
[9] | Spetch ML, Cheng K, MacDonald SE, Linkenhoker BA, Kelly DM, et al. (1997) Use of a landmark configuration in pigeons and humans: II. Generality across search tasks. J Comp Psychol 111: 14–24.
|
[10] | McGregor A, Jones PM, Good MA, Pearce JM (2006) Further evidence that rats rely on local rather than global spatial information to locate a hidden goal: reply to Cheng and Gallistel (2005). J Exp Psychol Anim B 32: 314–321.
|
[11] | Kelly DM, Chiandetti C, Vallortigara G (2011) Re-orienting in space: do animals use global or local geometry strategies? Biol Letters 7: 372–375.
|
[12] | Bodily KD, Eastman CK, Sturz BR (2011) Neither by global nor local cues alone: evidence for a unified orientation process. Anim Cogn 14: 655–674.
|
[13] | Sturz BR, Forloines MR, Bodily KD (2012) Enclosure size and the use of local and global geometric cues for reorientation. Psychon B Rev 19: 270–276.
|
[14] | Sturz BR, Gurley T, Bodily KD (2011) Orientation in trapezoid-shaped enclosures: implications for theoretical accounts of geometry learning. J Exp Psychol Anim B 37: 246–253.
|
[15] | Kelly DM, Durocher S (2011) Comparing geometric models for orientation: medial vs. principal axes. Commun Integr Biol 4: 710–712.
|
[16] | Burnham KP, Anderson DR (2002) Model selection and multi- model inference: a practical information-theoretic approach (2nd ed.). Springer-Verlag.
|
[17] | Sturz BR, Bodily KD (2011) Is surface-based orientation influenced by a proportional relationship of shape parameters? Psychon B Rev 18: 848–854.
|
[18] | Sturzl W, Cheung A, Cheng K, Ziel J (2008) The information content of panoramic images I: the rotational errors and the similarity of views in rectangular experimental arenas. J Exp Psychol Anim B 34: 1–14.
|
[19] | Cheung A, Sturzl W, Zeil J, Cheng K (2008) The information content of panoramic images II: view-based navigation in nonrectangular experimental arenas. J Exp Psychol Anim B 34: 15–30.
|
[20] | Graham TS, Cheng K (2009) Ants use the panoramic skyline as a visual cue during navigation. Curr Biol 19: R935–R937.
|
[21] | Wystrach A, Beugnon G, Cheng K (2011) Landmarks or panoramas: what do navigating ants attend to for guidance? Front Zool 8: 21.
|
[22] | Zeil J (2012) Visual homing: an insect perspective. Curr Opin Neurobiol 22: 285–293.
|
[23] | Lent DD, Graham P, Collett TS (2013) Visual scene perception in navigating wood ants. Curr Biol 23: 684–690.
|
[24] | Pecchia T, Gagliardo A, Vallortigara G (2011) Stable panoramic views facilitate snap-shot like memories for spatial reorientation in homing pigeons. PLoS ONE 6: e22657.
|
[25] | Pecchia T, Vallortigara G (2010) View-based strategy for reorientation by geometry. J Exp Biol 213: 2987–2996.
|
[26] | Epstein RA (2008) Parahippocampal and retrosplenial contributions to human spatial navigation. Trends Cogn Sci 12: 388–396.
|
[27] | Wystrach A, Graham P (2012) What can we learn from studies of insect navigation? Anim Behav 84: 13–20.
|
[28] | Lubyk DM, Dupuis B, Gutiérrez L, Spetch ML (2012) Geometric orientation by humans: angles weigh in. Psychon B Rev 19: 436–442.
|
[29] | Lubyk DM, Spetch ML (2012) Finding the best angle: pigeons (Columba livia) weight angular information more heavily than relative wall length in an open-field geometry task. Anim Cogn 15: 305–312.
|
[30] | Sandstrom NJ, Kaufman J, Huettel S (1998) Males and females use different distal cues in a virtual environment navigation task. Cognitive Brain Res 6: 351–360.
|
[31] | Kelly DM, Bischof WF (2005) Reorienting in images of a three-dimensional environment. J Exp Psychol – Hum L 31: 1391–1403.
|
[32] | Parsons TD, Larson P, Kratz K, Thiebaux M, Bluestein B, et al. (2004) Sex differences in mental rotation and spatial rotation in a virtual environment. Neuropsychologia 42: 555–562.
|
[33] | Vandenberg SG, Kuse AR (1978) Mental rotations, a group test of three-dimensional spatial visualization. Percept Motor Skill 47: 599–604.
|