全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...
PLOS ONE  2013 

Two Randomized Trials Provide No Consistent Evidence for Nonmusical Cognitive Benefits of Brief Preschool Music Enrichment

DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0082007

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Young children regularly engage in musical activities, but the effects of early music education on children's cognitive development are unknown. While some studies have found associations between musical training in childhood and later nonmusical cognitive outcomes, few randomized controlled trials (RCTs) have been employed to assess causal effects of music lessons on child cognition and no clear pattern of results has emerged. We conducted two RCTs with preschool children investigating the cognitive effects of a brief series of music classes, as compared to a similar but non-musical form of arts instruction (visual arts classes, Experiment 1) or to a no-treatment control (Experiment 2). Consistent with typical preschool arts enrichment programs, parents attended classes with their children, participating in a variety of developmentally appropriate arts activities. After six weeks of class, we assessed children's skills in four distinct cognitive areas in which older arts-trained students have been reported to excel: spatial-navigational reasoning, visual form analysis, numerical discrimination, and receptive vocabulary. We initially found that children from the music class showed greater spatial-navigational ability than did children from the visual arts class, while children from the visual arts class showed greater visual form analysis ability than children from the music class (Experiment 1). However, a partial replication attempt comparing music training to a no-treatment control failed to confirm these findings (Experiment 2), and the combined results of the two experiments were negative: overall, children provided with music classes performed no better than those with visual arts or no classes on any assessment. Our findings underscore the need for replication in RCTs, and suggest caution in interpreting the positive findings from past studies of cognitive effects of music instruction.

References

[1]  Flohr JW (2005) The musical lives of young children. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson.
[2]  Degé F, Kubicek C, Schwarzer G (2011) Music lessons and intelligence: A relation mediated by executive functions. Music Percept 29: 195–201.
[3]  Hanna-Pladdy B, MacKay A (2011) The relation between instrumental activity and cognitive aging. Neuropsychology 25: 378–386.
[4]  Elpus K (2013) Is it the music or is it selection bias? A nationwide analysis of music and non-music students' SAT scores. J Res Music Educ 61: 175–194.
[5]  Costa-Giomi E (1999) The effects of three years of piano instruction on children's cognitive development. J Res Music Educ 47: 198–212.
[6]  Bilhartz TD, Bruhn RA, Olson JE (1999) The effect of early music training on child cognitive development. J Appl Dev Psychol 20: 615–636.
[7]  Schellenberg EG (2004) Music lessons enhance IQ. Psychol Sci 15: 511–514.
[8]  Moreno S, Marques C, Santos A, Santos M, Castro S, et al. (2009) Musical training influences linguistic abilities in 8-year-old children: more evidence for brain plasticity. Cereb Cortex 19: 712–723.
[9]  Moreno S, Bialystok E, Barac R, Schellenberg EG, Capeda NJ, et al. (2011) Short-term music training enhances verbal intelligence and executive function. Psychol Sci 22: 1425–1433.
[10]  Wechsler D (2003) Wechsler intelligence scale for children, third edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp.
[11]  Wechsler D (2002) Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence—third edition (WPPSI-III): Technical and interpretive manual. San Antonio: Pearson.
[12]  Beggs DL, Mouw JT (1980) Developing cognitive abilities test. Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press.
[13]  Thorndike R, Hagen E, Sattler J (1986) The Stanford-Binet intelligence scale, fourth edition. Chicago: Riverside.
[14]  Hartshorne JK, Schachner A (2012) Tracking replicability as a method of post-publication open evaluation. Front Comput Neurosci 6: 1–14.
[15]  Button KS, Ioannidis JPA, Mokrysz C, Nosek BA, Flint J, et al. (2013) Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 14: 365–376.
[16]  Dunn LM, Dunn LM (1997) Peabody picture vocabulary test, third edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services.
[17]  Sternberg RJ (1985) Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intelligence. NY: Cambridge University Press.
[18]  McDermott PA, Fantuzzo JW, Glutting JJ, Watkins MW, Baggaley AR (1992) Illusions of meaning in the ipsative assessment of children's ability. J Spec Educ 25: 504–526.
[19]  McDermott PA, Fantuzzo JW, Glutting JJ (1990) Just say no to subtest analysis: a critique on Wechsler theory and practice. J Psychoeduc Assess 8: 290–302.
[20]  Sattler JM (2001) Assessment of children: Cognitive applications. La Mesa, CA: Sattler Publishing.
[21]  Kaufman A (1994) Intelligent testing with the WISC-III. NY: Wiley.
[22]  Schellenberg EG (2006) Long-term positive associations between music lessons and IQ. J Educ Psychol 98: 457–468.
[23]  Halberda J, Ly R, Wilmer JB, Naiman DQ, Germine L (2012) Number sense across the lifespan as revealed by a massive internet-based sample. P Natl Acad Sci USA 109: 11116–11120.
[24]  Libertus ME, Feigenson L, Halberda J (2011) Preschool acuity of the approximate number system correlates with school math ability. Developmental Sci 14: 1292–1300.
[25]  Gilmore CK, McCarthy SE, Spelke ES (2010) Non-symbolic arithmetic abilities and mathematics achievement in the first year of formal schooling. Cognition 115: 394–406.
[26]  Halberda J, Mazzocco MMM, Feigenson L (2008) Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement. Nature 455: 665–668.
[27]  Piazza M, Pica P, Izard V, Spelke ES, Dehaene S (2013) Education enhances the acuity of the non-verbal approximate number system. Psychol Sci: In press.
[28]  Spelke ES (2008) Effects of music instruction on developing cognitive systems at the foundations of mathematics and science. In: Asbury C, Rich B, editors. Learning, arts, and the brain: The Dana Consortium report on arts and cognition. New York/Washington, D.C.: Dana Press. pp. 17–49.
[29]  Izard V, Pica P, Dehaene S, Hinchey D, Spelke ES (2011) Geometry as a universal mental construction. In: Brannon E, Dehaene S, editors. Space, time and number in the brain: Searching for the foundations of mathematical thought. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 319–332.
[30]  MENC: The National Association for Music Education (2000) Spotlight on early childhood music education: Selected articles from state MEA journals. Reston, VA: Author.
[31]  Deater-Deckard K, Petrill SA, Thompson LA, DeThorne LS (2005) A cross-sectional behavioral genetic analysis of task persistence in the transition to middle childhood. Developmental Sci 8: F21–F26.
[32]  McClelland MM, Acock AC, Piccinin A, Rhea SA, Stallings MC (2013) Relations between preschool attention span-persistence and age 25 educational outcomes. Early Child Res Q 28: 314–234.
[33]  Gordon EE (1989) Advanced measures of music audiation. Chicago: G.I.A. Publications, Inc.
[34]  Fox DB (1989) MusicTIME and music times two: The Eastman infant-toddler music programs. In: Andress B, editor. Promising practices: Prekindergarten music education. Reston, VA: Music Educators National Conference. pp. 12–24.
[35]  Fox DB (1991) Music classes for infants and toddlers. Am Music Teacher 40: 20–23.
[36]  Mehr SA, Fox DB (2010 July) Nurturing musical lives of families through shared experience: Music Times Two. Poster session at the Early Childhood Music Education Commission of the International Society for Music Education, Beijing, China.
[37]  National Association for the Education of Young Children (2009) Developmentally appropriate practice in early childhood programs serving children from birth through age 8. Available: http://www.naeyc.org/positionstatements/?dap Accessed 2013 October 31.
[38]  Swears L (1998) Kindermusik for the young child: introductory information for young child curriculum. Greensboro, NC: Kindermusik International, Inc.
[39]  Guilmartin K (2000) Early childhood music education in the new millennium. Am Music Teacher 49: 40–41.
[40]  Orff C, Keetman G, Regner H (1982) Music for children: Orff-Schulwerk series. Ann Arbor, MI: Schott Music Corp.
[41]  Consortium of National Arts Education Associations (1994) National standards for arts education: what every young American should know and be able to do in the arts. Reston, VA: Music Educators National Conference.
[42]  Eastman Community Music School website. Available: http://www.esm.rochester.edu/community/. Accessed 2013 October 31.
[43]  Feierabend JM (1996) Music and movement for infants and toddlers: naturally wonder-full. Ear Child Connections 2: 19–26.
[44]  Berger AA, Cooper S (2003) Musical play: a case study of preschool children and parents. J Res Music Educ 51: 151–166.
[45]  Schiller M (1995) Reggio Emilia: A focus on emergent curriculum and art. Art Educ 48(3): , 45–50.
[46]  Shusterman A, Lee SA, Spelke ES (2008) Young children's spontaneous use of geometry in maps. Developmental Sci 11: F1–F7.
[47]  Izard V, Spelke ES (2009) Development of sensitivity to geometry in visual forms. Hum Evol 23: 213–248.
[48]  Winner E, Hetland L (1999 March) Mozart and the SATs. . The New York Times p. 25.
[49]  Gordon EE (1988) Musical aptitude profile. Chicago: Riverside Publishing Co.
[50]  Bruininks RH (1978) Bruininks-Oseretsky test of motor proficiency. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Center.
[51]  Canadian Test Centre, Inc. (1992) Canadian achievement tests, second edition. Markham, ON: Author.
[52]  Coopersmith S (1981) Coopersmith self-esteem inventories. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
[53]  Chosky L (1999) The Kodàly method I: comprehensive music education. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
[54]  Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL (1985) Kaufman test of educational achievement. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
[55]  Reynolds CR, Kamphaus RW (1992) Behavior assessment system for children. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
[56]  Cumming G, Fidler F (2010) Effect sizes and confidence intervals. In: Hancock GR, Mueller RO, editors. The reviewer's guide to quantitative methods in the social sciences. New York: Routledge. pp. 79–92.
[57]  Wuytack J, Palheiros GB (1995) Audi??o musical active [Active musical audition]. Portugal: Associa??o Wuytack de Pedagogia Musical.
[58]  Wechsler D (2003) Escala de inteligência de Wechsler para Crian?as-3a edi?ao. Lisboa, Portugal: Cegoc.
[59]  Succena A, Castro S (2010) Bateria de avalia??o da leitura em Português Europeu. Lisboa, Portugal: CEGOC.
[60]  Marques C, Moreno S, Castro SL, Besson M (2007) Musicians detect pitch violation in a foreign language better than non-musicians: behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. J Cogn Neurosci 19: 1453–1463.

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133