[1] | Galton F (1880) Visualised numerals. Nature 21: 252–256.
|
[2] | De Hevia MD, Vallar G, Girelli L (2008) Visualizing numbers in the mind’s eye: the role of visuo-spatial processes in numerical abilities. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32: 1361–1372.
|
[3] | Fias W, Fischer MH (2005) Spatial Representation of Numbers. In: Campbell JID, editor. Handbook of Mathematical Cognition. New York: Psychology Press. pp. 43-54.
|
[4] | Hubbard EM, Piazza M, Pinel P, Dehaene S (2005) Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex. Nat Revi Neurosci 6: 435–448.
|
[5] | Dehaene S, Dupoux E, Mehler J (1990) Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 16: 626–641.
|
[6] | Dehaene S, Bossini S, Giraux P (1993) The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. J Exp Psychol Gen 122: 371–396.
|
[7] | Restle F (1970) Speed of adding and comparing numbers. J Exp Psychol 83: 274–278.
|
[8] | Gevers W, Caessens B, Fias W (2005) Towards a common processing architecture underlying Simon and SNARC effects. Eur J Cogn Psychol 17: 659–673.
|
[9] | Gevers W, Ratinckx E, De Baene W, Fias W (2006) Further evidence that the SNARC effect is processed along a dual-route architecture: Evidence from the lateralized readiness potential. Exp Psychol 53: 58–68.
|
[10] | Gevers W, Verguts T, Reynvoet B, Caessens B, Fias W (2006) Numbers and space: a computational model of the SNARC effect. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 32: 32–44.
|
[11] | Gevers W, Santens S, Dhooge E, Chen Q, Van den Bossche L, et al. (2010) Verbal-spatial and visuospatial coding of number-space interactions. J Exp Psychol Gen 139: 180–190.
|
[12] | Santens S, Gevers W (2008) The SNARC effect does not imply a mental number line. Cognition 108: 263–270.
|
[13] | Van Dijck J-P, Gevers W, Lafosse C, Fias W (2011) The heterogeneous nature of number-space interactions. Front Hum Neurosci 5: 182.
|
[14] | Chen Q, Verguts T (2010) Beyond the mental number line: A neural network model of number-space interactions. Cogn Psychol 60: 218–240.
|
[15] | Proctor RW, Cho YS (2006) Polarity correspondence: A general principle for performance of speeded binary classification tasks. Psychol Bull 132: 416–442.
|
[16] | Keus IM, Schwarz W (2005) Searching for the functional locus of the SNARC effect: evidence for a response-related origin. Mem Cognit 33: 681–695.
|
[17] | Keus IM, Jenks KM, Schwarz W (2005) Psychophysiological evidence that the SNARC effect has its functional locus in a response selection stage. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 24: 48–56.
|
[18] | Fias W, van Dijck J-P, Gevers W (2011) How number is associated with space? : the role of working memory. In: Dehaene S, Brannon E, editors. Space, time and number in the brain: searching for the foundations of mathematical thought. Amsterdam: Elsevier. pp. 133–148.
|
[19] | Van Dijck J-P, Fias W (2011) A working memory account for spatial-numerical associations. Cognition 119: 114–119.
|
[20] | Previtali P, de Hevia MD, Girelli L (2010) Placing order in space: the SNARC effect in serial learning. Exp Brain Res 201: 599–605.
|
[21] | Fias W, Brysbaert M, Geypens F, d’ Ydewalle G (1996) The importance of magnitude information in numerical processing: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Mathematical Cognition 2: 95–110.
|
[22] | Ren P, Nicholls MER, Ma Y, Chen L (2011) Size matters: non-numerical magnitude affects the spatial coding of response. PloS One 6: e23553.
|
[23] | Wood G, Fischer MH (2008) Numbers, space, and action - from finger counting to the mental number line and beyond. Cortex 44: 353–358.
|
[24] | Wood G, Willmes K, Nuerk H-C, Fischer MH (2008) On the cognitive link between space and number: A meta-analysis of the SNARC effect. Psychol Sci Q 50: 489–525.
|
[25] | Wood G, Nuerk H-C, Willmes K (2006) Crossed hands and the SNARC effect: a failure to replicate Dehaene, Bossini and Giraux (1993). Cortex 42: 1069–1079 discussion 1119–1123.
|
[26] | Wood G, Nuerk H-C, Willmes K (2006) Variability of the Snarc Effect: Systematic Interindividual Differences or Just Random Error? Cortex 42: 1119–1123.
|
[27] | Cohen Kadosh R, Henik A (2007) Can synaesthesia research inform cognitive science? Trends Cogn Sci 11: 177–184.
|
[28] | Di Luca S, Granà A, Semenza C, Seron X, Pesenti M (2006) Finger-digit compatibility in Arabic numeral processing. Q J Exp Psychol 59: 1648–1663.
|
[29] | Fischer MH (2008) Finger counting habits modulate spatial-numerical associations. Cortex 44: 386–392.
|
[30] | Tschentscher N, Hauk O, Fischer MH, Pulvermüller F (2012) You can count on the motor cortex: finger counting habits modulate motor cortex activation evoked by numbers. Neuroimage 59: 3139–3148.
|
[31] | Cipora K, Nuerk H-C (2013) Is the SNARC effect related to the level of mathematics? No systematic relationship observed despite more power, more repetitions, and more direct assessment of arithmetic skill. Q J Exp Psychol. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2013.772215.
|
[32] | Bull R, Cleland AA, Mitchell T (2013) Sex Differences in the Spatial Representation of Number. J Exp Psychol Gen 142: 181–192.
|
[33] | Fischer MH, Castel AD, Dodd MD, Pratt J (2003) Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention. Nat Neurosci 6: 555–556.
|
[34] | Casarotti M, Michielin M, Zorzi M, Umiltà C (2007) Temporal order judgment reveals how number magnitude affects visuospatial attention. Cognition 102: 101–117.
|
[35] | Galfano G, Rusconi E, Umiltà C (2006) Number magnitude orients attention, but not against one’s will. Psychon Bull Rev 13: 869–874.
|
[36] | Ristic J, Wright A, Kingstone A (2006) The number line effect reflects top-down control. Psychon Bull Rev 13: 862–868.
|
[37] | Bonato M, Priftis K, Marenzi R, Zorzi M (2009) Normal and impaired reflexive orienting of attention after central nonpredictive cues. J Cogn Neurosci 21: 745–759.
|
[38] | Castronovo J, G?bel SM (2012) Impact of high mathematics education on the number sense. PloS One 7: e33832.
|
[39] | Dewind NK, Brannon EM (2012) Malleability of the approximate number system: effects of feedback and training. Front Hum Neurosci Vol 6 - Article 68.
|
[40] | Nys J, Ventura P, Fernandes T, Querido L, Leybaert J, et al. (2013) Does math education modify the approximate number system? A comparison of schooled and unschooled adults. Trends Neurosci Educ 2: 13–22.
|
[41] | Piazza M, Pica P, Izard V, Spelke ES, Dehaene S (2013) Education Enhances the Acuity of the Nonverbal Approximate Number System. Psychol Sci 24: 1037–1043.
|
[42] | Gilmore CK, McCarthy SE, Spelke ES (2010) Non-symbolic arithmetic abilities and mathematics achievement in the first year of formal schooling. Cognition 115: 394–406.
|
[43] | Mazzocco MMM, Feigenson L, Halberda J (2011) Preschoolers’ precision of the approximate number system predicts later school mathematics performance. PloS One 6: e23749.
|
[44] | Mejias S, Schiltz C (2013) Estimation abilities of large numerosities in Kindergartners. Front Psychol 4: 518.
|
[45] | Mussolin C, Nys J, Leybaert J, Content A (2012) Relationships between approximate number system acuity and early symbolic number abilities. Trends Neurosci Educ 1: 21–31.
|
[46] | Booth JL, Siegler RS (2006) Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical estimation. Dev Psychol 42: 189–201.
|
[47] | Booth JL, Siegler RS (2008) Numerical magnitude representations influence arithmetic learning. Child Dev 79: 1016–1031.
|
[48] | Siegler RS, Booth JL (2004) Development of numerical estimation in young children. Child Dev 75: 428–444.
|
[49] | Fischer U, Moeller K, Bientzle M, Cress U, Nuerk H-C (2011) Sensori-motor spatial training of number magnitude representation. Psychon Bull Rev 18: 177–183.
|
[50] | Bachot J, Gevers W, Fias W, Roeyers H (2005) Number sense in children with visuo-spatial disabilities: orientation of the mental number line. Psychol Sci Q 47: 172–183.
|
[51] | Schweiter M, Weinhold Zulauf M, von Aster M (2005) Die Entwicklung r?umlicher Zahlenrepr?sentationen und Rechenfertigkeiten bei Kindern. Z Neuropsychologie 16: 105–113.
|
[52] | Hoffmann D, Hornung C, Martin R, Schiltz C (2013) Developing number-space associations: SNARC effects using a colour-discrimination task in 5 year olds. J Exp Child Psychol 116: 775–791.
|
[53] | Ashkenazi S, Henik A (2010) A disassociation between physical and mental number bisection in developmental dyscalculia. Neuropsychologia 48: 2861–2868.
|
[54] | Landerl K, Bevan A, Butterworth B (2004) Developmental dyscalculia and basic numerical capacities: a study of 8-9-year-old students. Cognition 93: 99–125.
|
[55] | Mejias S, Mussolin C, Rousselle L, Grégoire J, No?l M-P (2012) Numerical and nonnumerical estimation in children with and without mathematical learning disabilities. Child Neuropsychol 18: 550–575.
|
[56] | Mussolin C, Martin R, Schiltz C (2011) Relationships between number and space processing in adults with and without dyscalculia. Acta Psychol 138: 193–203.
|
[57] | Mussolin C, Mejias S, No?l M-P (2010) Symbolic and nonsymbolic number comparison in children with and without dyscalculia. Cognition 115: 10–25.
|
[58] | Rubinsten O, Henik A (2005) Automatic activation of internal magnitudes: a study of developmental dyscalculia. Neuropsychology 19: 641–648.
|
[59] | Butterworth B (2005) Developmental Dyscalculia. In: Campbell JID, editor. Handbook of Mathematical Cognition. New York: Psychology Press. pp. 455–467.
|
[60] | Dehaene S (1997) The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. USA: Oxford University Press. 288 p.
|
[61] | Mejias S, Grégoire J, No?l M-P (2012) Numerical estimation in adults with and without developmental dyscalculia. Learning Individ Differ 22: 164–170.
|
[62] | De Smedt B, Gilmore CK (2011) Defective number module or impaired access? Numerical magnitude processing in first graders with mathematical difficulties. J Exp Child Psychol 108: 278–292.
|
[63] | Rousselle L, No?l M-P (2007) Basic numerical skills in children with mathematics learning disabilities: a comparison of symbolic vs non-symbolic number magnitude processing. Cognition 102: 361–395.
|
[64] | Rubinsten O, Henik A (2006) Double dissociation of functions in developmental dyslexia and dyscalculia. J Educ Psychol 98: 854–867.
|
[65] | Censabella S, No?l M-P (2004) Interference in arithmetic facts: Are active suppression processes involved when performing simple mental arithmetic? Curr Psychol Cogn 22: 635–671.
|
[66] | De Visscher A, No?l M-P (2013) A case study of arithmetic facts dyscalculia caused by a hypersensitivity-to-interference in memory. Cortex 49: 50–70.
|
[67] | Geary DC (1993) Mathematical disabilities: cognitive, neuropsychological, and genetic components. Psychol Bull 114: 345–362.
|
[68] | Geary DC, Hoard MK, Byrd-Craven J, Nugent L, Numtee C (2007) Cognitive mechanisms underlying achievement deficits in children with mathematical learning disability. Child Dev 78: 1343–1359.
|
[69] | Geary DC (2004) Mathematics and learning disabilities. J Learn Disabil 37: 4–15.
|
[70] | Passolunghi MC, Siegel LS (2004) Working memory and access to numerical information in children with disability in mathematics. J Exp Child Psychol 88: 348–367.
|
[71] | Zhang H, Wu H (2011) Inhibitory ability of children with developmental dyscalculia. J Huazhong Univ Sci Technolog Med Sci 31: 131–136.
|
[72] | Gut M, Szumska I, Wasilewska M, Ja?kowski P (2012) Are low and high number magnitudes processed differently while resolving the conflict evoked by the SNARC effect? Int J Psychophysiol 85: 7–16.
|
[73] | Zbrodoff NJ (1995) Why is 9 + 7 harder than 2 + 3? Strength and interference as explanations of the problem-size effect. Mem Cognit 23: 689–700.
|
[74] | Anderson J, Reder L (1999) The fan effect: New results and new theories. J Exp Psychol Gen 128: 186–197.
|
[75] | De Smedt B, Verschaffel L, Ghesquière P (2009) The predictive value of numerical magnitude comparison for individual differences in mathematics achievement. J Exp Child Psychol 103: 469–479.
|
[76] | Holloway ID, Ansari D (2009) Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: the numerical distance effect and individual differences in children’s mathematics achievement. J Exp Child Psychol 103: 17–29.
|
[77] | Hasher L, Zacks RT (1979) Automatic and effortful processes in memory. J Exp Psychol Gen 108: 356–388.
|
[78] | Lavie N (2005) Distracted and confused?: selective attention under load. Trends Cogn Sci 9: 75–82.
|
[79] | Fischer MH, Rottmann J (2005) Do negative numbers have a place on the mental number line. Psychol Sci Q 47: 22–32.
|
[80] | Schneider W, Eschmann A, Zuccolotto A (2002) E-prime User’s Guide. Pittsburgh, Psychology Software Tools Inc.
|
[81] | Shalev RS, Manor O, Kerem B, Ayali M, Badichi N, et al. (2001) Developmental dyscalculia is a familial learning disability. J Learn Disabil 34: 59–65.
|
[82] | Della Sala S, Gray C, Baddeley A, Allamano N, Wilson L (1999) Pattern span: a tool for unwelding visuo-spatial memory. Neuropsychologia 37: 1189–1199.
|
[83] | Della Sala S, Gray C, Baddeley AD, Wilson L (1997) Visual Pattern Test. Bury St Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company.
|
[84] | Nuerk H-C, Iversen W, Willmes K (2004) Notational modulation of the SNARC and the MARC (linguistic markedness of response codes) effect. Q J Exp Psychol 57: 835–836.
|
[85] | Lorch RF, Myers JL (1990) Regression analyses of repeated measures data in cognitive research. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 16: 149–157.
|
[86] | Pinhas M, Tzelgov J, Ganor-Stern D (2012) Estimating linear effects in ANOVA designs: the easy way. Behav Res Methods 44: 788–794.
|
[87] | Tzelgov J, Zohar-Shai B, Nuerk HC (2013) On defining quantifying and measuring the SNARC effect. Front Psychol. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00302
|
[88] | Pinhas M, Tzelgov J (2012) Expanding on the Mental Number Line: Zero Is Perceived as the ‘Smallest’. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 38: 1187–1205.
|
[89] | G?bel SM, Shaki S, Fischer MH (2011) The Cultural Number Line: A Review of Cultural and Linguistic Influences on the Development of Number Processing. J Cross Cult Psychol 42: 543–565.
|
[90] | Shaki S, Fischer MH, Petrusic WM (2009) Reading habits for both words and numbers contribute to the SNARC effect. Psychon Bull Rev 16: 328–331.
|
[91] | Bonato M, Fabbri S, Umiltà C, Zorzi M (2007) The mental representation of numerical fractions: real or integer? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 33: 1410–9.
|
[92] | Schneider M, Grabner RH, Paetsch J (2009) Mental number line, number line estimation, and mathematical achievement: Their interrelations in grades 5 and 6. J Educ Psychol 101: 359–372.
|
[93] | Wood G, Stiglbauer B, Kaufmann L, Fischer M, Nuerk H-C (2010) Age effect on the spatial-numerical associations. Personal communication.
|
[94] | Hoffmann D, Pigat D, Schiltz C (under revision) The impact of inhibition capacities on number-space associations in adults.
|
[95] | Ashcraft MH, Faust MW (1994) Mathematics anxiety and mental arithmetic performance: An exploratory investigation. Cogn Emot 8: 97–125.
|
[96] | Faust MW, Ashcraft MH, Fleck DE (1996) Mathematics Anxiety Effects in Simple and Complex Addition. Mathematical Cognition 2: 25–62.
|
[97] | Ashcraft MH, Kirk EP (2001) The relationships among working memory, math anxiety, and performance. J Exp Psychol Gen 130: 224–237.
|
[98] | Eysenck MW, Derakshan N, Santos R, Calvo MG (2007) Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion 7: 336–353.
|
[99] | Ashcraft MH (2002) Math Anxiety: Personal, Educational, and Cognitive Consequences. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 11: 181–185.
|
[100] | Imbo I, Brauwer JD, Fias W, Gevers W (2012) The development of the SNARC effect: evidence for early verbal coding. J Exp Child Psychol 111: 671–680.
|
[101] | Van Dijck J-P, Gevers W, Fias W (2009) Numbers are associated with different types of spatial information depending on the task. Cognition 113 248–253.
|
[102] | Delazer M, Domahs F, Bartha L, Brenneis C, Lochy A, et al. (2003) Learning complex arithmetic-an fMRI study. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 18: 76–88.
|
[103] | Dehaene S, Spelke E, Pinel P, Stanescu R, Tsivkin S (1999) Sources of mathematical thinking: behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. Science 284: 970–974.
|
[104] | Grabner RH, Ansari D, Reishofer G, Stern E, Ebner F, et al. (2007) Individual differences in mathematical competence predict parietal brain activation during mental calculation. Neuroimage 38: 346–356.
|
[105] | Grabner RH, Ansari D, Koschutnig K, Reishofer G, Ebner F, et al. (2009) To retrieve or to calculate? Left angular gyrus mediates the retrieval of arithmetic facts during problem solving. Neuropsychologia 47: 604–608.
|
[106] | Grabner RH, Ischebeck A, Reishofer G, Koschutnig K, Delazer M, et al. (2009) Fact learning in complex arithmetic and figural-spatial tasks: the role of the angular gyrus and its relation to mathematical competence. Hum Brain Mapp 30: 2936–2952.
|
[107] | Cutini S, Scarpa F, Scatturin P, Dell’acqua R, Zorzi M (2012) Number-Space Interactions in the Human Parietal Cortex: Enlightening the SNARC Effect with Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy. Cereb Cortex. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs321.
|
[108] | Dehaene S, Piazza M, Pinel P, Cohen L (2003) Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cogn Neuropsychol 20: 487–506.
|
[109] | Knops A, Thirion B, Hubbard EM, Michel V, Dehaene S (2009) Recruitment of an area involved in eye movements during mental arithmetic. Science 324: 1583–1585.
|
[110] | Koten JW Jr, Lonnemann J, Willmes K, Knops A (2011) Micro and macro pattern analyses of FMRI data support both early and late interaction of numerical and spatial information. Front Hum Neurosci 5: 115.
|
[111] | Herrera A, Macizo P, Semenza C (2008) The role of working memory in the association between number magnitude and space. Acta Psychol 128: 225–237.
|