全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...
PLOS ONE  2014 

When We Should Worry More: Using Cognitive Bias Modification to Drive Adaptive Health Behaviour

DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0085092

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

A lack of behavioural engagement in health promotion or disease prevention is a problem across many health domains. In these cases where people face a genuine danger, a reduced focus on threat and low levels of anxiety or worry are maladaptive in terms of promoting protection or prevention behaviour. Therefore, it is possible that increasing the processing of threat will increase worry and thereby enhance engagement in adaptive behaviour. Laboratory studies have shown that cognitive bias modification (CBM) can increase or decrease anxiety and worry when increased versus decreased processing of threat is encouraged. In the current study, CBM for interpretation (CBM-I) is used to target engagement in sun protection behaviour. The goal was to investigate whether inducing a negative rather than a positive interpretation bias for physical threat information can enhance worry elicited when viewing a health campaign video (warning against melanoma skin cancer), and consequently lead to more adaptive behaviour (sun protection). Participants were successfully trained to either adopt a positive or negative interpretation bias using physical threat scenarios. However, contrary to expectations results showed that participants in the positive training condition reported higher levels of worry elicited by the melanoma video than participants in the negative training condition. Video elicited worry was, however, positively correlated with a measure of engagement in sun protection behaviour, suggesting that higher levels of worry do promote adaptive behaviour. These findings imply that more research is needed to determine under which conditions increased versus decreased processing of threat can drive adaptive worry. Various potential explanations for the current findings and suggestions for future research are discussed.

References

[1]  Lucas R, Prüss-üstün A, McMichael T, Organization WH, Smith W, et al. (2006) Solar Ultraviolet Radiation: World Health Organization.
[2]  Allen S, Zhu S, Sauter C, Layde P, Hargarten S (2006) A comprehensive statewide analysis of seatbelt non-use with injury and hospital admissions: new data, old problem. Acad Emerg Med 13: 427–434.
[3]  Witte K, Allen M (2000) A meta-analysis of fear appeals: Implications for effective public health campaigns. Health Educ Behav 27: 591–615.
[4]  McCaul KD, Branstetter AD, Schroeder DM, Glasgow RE (1996) What is the relationship between breast cancer risk and mammography screening? A meta-analytic review. Health Psychol 15: 423–429.
[5]  MacLeod C (2012) Cognitive bias modification procedures in the management of mental disorders. Curr Opin Psychiatry 25: 114–120.
[6]  MacLeod C, Rutherford E, Campbell L, Ebsworthy G, Holker L (2002) Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: Assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. J Abnorm Psychol 111: 107–123.
[7]  Mathews A, Mackintosh B (2000) Induced emotional interpretation bias and anxiety. J Abnorm Psychol 109: 602–615.
[8]  MacLeod C, Clarke PJF (2013) Cognitive Bias Modification: A New Frontier in Cognition and Emotion Research. In: Robinson MD, Watkins ER, Harmon-Jones E, editors. Handbook of Cognition and Emotion. New York: Guilford. pp. 540–562.
[9]  Ohman A, Mineka S (2001) Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychol Rev 108: 483–522.
[10]  Hirsch CR, Hayes S, Mathews A (2009) Looking on the bright side: Accessing benign meanings reduces worry. J Abnorm Psychol 118: 44–54.
[11]  Hayes S, Hirsch CR, Krebs G, Mathews A (2010) The effects of modifying interpretation bias on worry in generalized anxiety disorder. Behav Res Ther 48: 171–178.
[12]  Hoppitt L, Mathews A, Yiend J, Mackintosh B (2010) Cognitive Bias Modification: The Critical Role of Active Training in Modifying Emotional Responses. Behav Ther 41: 73–81.
[13]  Webb TL, Sheeran P (2006) Does changing behavioral intentions engender bahaviour change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychol Bull 132: 249–268.
[14]  Verkuil B, Brosschot JF, Thayer JF (2007) Capturing worry in daily life: Are trait questionnaires sufficient? Behav Res Ther 45: 1835–1844.
[15]  Spielberger C, Gorsuch R, Lushene R, Vagg P, Jacobs G (1983) Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory STAI (Form Y): Self-evaluation questionnaire. Alto: Ca: Consulting Psychologist Press.
[16]  Barnes LL, Harp D, Jung WS (2002) Reliability generalization of scores on the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Edu Psycholl Meas 62: 603–618.
[17]  Stober J, Joormann J (2001) A short form of the Worry Domains Questionnaire: construction and factorial validation. Pers Individ Dif 31: 591–598.
[18]  Cancer Council Australia Website PSC. http://www.cancer.org.au Accessed 15 Dec 2012
[19]  Tukey JW (1977) Exploratory Data Analysis: Addison-Wesley.
[20]  Fazio RH (1990) A practical guide to the use of response latency in social psychological research. In: Hendrick C, Clark MS, editors. Review of Personality and Social Psychology. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications. pp. 74–97.
[21]  Mikulincer M (1996) Mental rumination and learned helplessness: Cognitive shifts during helplessness training and their behavioral consequences. In: Sarason IG, Pierce GR, Sarason BR, editors. Cognitive interference: Theories, methods, and findings. Mahwah: NJ: Erlbaum. pp. 191–209.
[22]  Ciarocco NJ, Vohs KD, Baumeister RF (2010) Some good news about rumination: task-focused thinking after failure facilitates performance improvement. J Soc Clin Psychol 29: 1057–1073.
[23]  Mackintosh B, Mathews A, Eckstein D, Hoppitt L (2013) Specificity effects in the modification of interpretation bias and stress reactivity. J Exp Psychopathol 4: 133–147.
[24]  Mermelstein R, Weeks K, Turner L, Cobb J (1999) When tailored feedback backfires: A skin cancer prevention intervention for adolescents. Canc Res Ther Contr 8: 69–79.
[25]  De Rooij MJM, Rampen FHJ, Schouten LJ, Neumann HAM (1997) Factors influencing participation among melanoma screening attenders. Acta Derm Venereol 77: 467–470.
[26]  McCaul KD, Mullens AB (2003) Affect, thought, and self-protective health behavior: The case of worry and cancer screening. Soc Psychol Foundation of Health and Illness: 137–168.
[27]  Rivkin ID, Taylor SE (1999) The effects of mental simulation on coping with controllable stressful events. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 25: 1451–1462.
[28]  Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A (2007) G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods 39: 175–191.
[29]  Clarke PJF, Nanthakumar S, Notebaert L, Holmes EA, Blackwell SE, et al. (2013) Simply Imagining Sunshine, Lollipops and Rainbows Will Not Budge the Bias: The Role of Ambiguity in Interpretive Bias Modification. Cognit Ther Res.
[30]  Teachman BA, Addison LM (2008) Training Non-Threatening Interpretations in Spider Fear. Cognit Ther Res 32: 448–459.
[31]  Lange W-G, Salemink E, Windey I, Keijsers GPJ, Krans J, et al. (2010) Does Modified Interpretation Bias Influence Automatic Avoidance Behaviour? Appl Cogn Psychol 24: 326–337.
[32]  Pictet A, Coughtrey AE, Mathews A, Holmes EA (2011) Fishing for happiness: The effects of generating positive imagery on mood and behaviour. Behav Res Ther 49: 885–891.
[33]  Dandeneau SD, Baldwin MW, Baccus JR, Sakellaropoulo M, Pruessner JC (2007) Cutting stress off at the pass: Reducing vigilance and responsiveness to social threat by manipulating attention. J Pers Soc Psychol 93: 651–666.

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133