全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Mill and Mental Phenomena: Critical Contributions to a Science of Cognition

DOI: 10.3390/bs3020217

Keywords: John Stuart Mill, attention, meta-cognition, cognitive neuroscience, cognitive therapy

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Attempts to define cognition preceded John Stuart Mill’s life and continue to this day. John Stuart Mill envisioned a science of mental phenomena informed by associationism, empirical introspection, and neurophysiology, and he advanced specific ideas that still influence modern conceptions of cognition. The present article briefly reviews Mill’s personal history and the times in which he lived, and it traces the evolution of ideas that have run through him to contemporary cognitive concepts. The article also highlights contemporary problems in defining cognition and supports specific criteria regarding what constitutes cognition.

References

[1]  Mill, J.S. A system of logic. In Excerpt reprinted in A History of Psychology, 3rd; Benjamin, L.T., Ed.; Blackwell Publishing: Malden, MA, USA, 2009; pp. 39–44.
[2]  Cognition. Oxford English Dictionary; Oxford University: Oxford, UK, 2012. Available online: http://www.oed.com/view/Entry/35876?redirectedFrom=cognition#eid (accessed on 20 December 2012).
[3]  Chaney, D.W. An overview of the first use of the terms cognition and behavior. Behav. Sci. 2013, 3, 143–153, doi:10.3390/bs3010143.
[4]  Walter, S.; K?stner, L. The where and what of cognition: The untenability of cognitive agnosticism and the limits of the Motley Crew Argument. Cogn. Syst. Res. 2012, 13, 12–23, doi:10.1016/j.cogsys.2010.10.001.
[5]  Stone, T.; Davies, M. Theoretical issues in cognitive psychology. In Cognitive Psychology, 2nd; Braisby, N., Gellatly, A., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2012.
[6]  Anderson, J.R. Cognitive Psychology and its Implications, 6th ed.; Worth Publishers: New York, NY, USA, 2005.
[7]  Adams, F. Why we still need a mark of the cognitive. Cogn. Syst. Res. 2010, 11, 324–331, doi:10.1016/j.cogsys.2010.03.001.
[8]  Damasio, A. Descartes Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain; Penguin Books: New York, NY, USA, 1994.
[9]  Habibi, D.A. John Stuart Mill and the Ethic of Human Growth; Kluwer Academic Publishers: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2001.
[10]  Capaldi, N. John Stuart Mill: A Biography; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2004.
[11]  Mill, J.S. Autobiography, 1873. Available online: http://www.bartleby.com/25/1/ (accessed on 20 December 2012).
[12]  Wilson, F. John Stuart Mill. In The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy; Zalta, E.N., Ed.; Stanford University: Stanford, CA, USA, 2007.
[13]  Kuhn, T.S. The Structure of Scientific Revolutions; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1962.
[14]  Comte, A. The Positive Philosophy of Auguste Comte; Calvin Blanchard: New York, NY, USA, 1855.
[15]  Gibson, E.J. Has psychology a future? Psychol. Sci. 1994, 5, 69–76, doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00633.x.
[16]  Moore, T.V. Cognitive Psychology; J. B. Lippincott & Company: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 1939.
[17]  Mathews, A.; Mackintosh, B. A cognitive model of selective processing in anxiety. Cogn. Ther. Res. 1998, 22, 539–560, doi:10.1023/A:1018738019346.
[18]  Damasio, A. The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness; Harcourt, Inc.: San Diego, CA, USA, 1999.
[19]  Wilson, F. Mill on psychology and the moral sciences. In The Cambridge Companion to Mill; Skorupski, J.M., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1998.
[20]  Schmidgen, H. Wundt as chemist? A fresh look at his practice and theory of experimentation. Am. J. Psychol. 2003, 116, 469–476, doi:10.2307/1423504.
[21]  Mandler, G. Origins of the cognitive (r)evolution. J. Hist. Behav. Sci. 2002, 38, 339–353, doi:10.1002/jhbs.10066.
[22]  Barrouillet, P.; Bernardin, S.; Portrat, S.; Vergauwe, E.; Camos, V. Time and cognitive load in working memory. J. Exp. Psychol-Learn Mem. Cogn. 2007, 33, 570–585, doi:10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.570.
[23]  Mill, J.S. August Comte and Positivism, 1865. Available online: http://www.gutenberg.org/cache/epub/16833/pg16833.txt (accessed on 20 December 2012).
[24]  Plunkett, K.; Juola, P. A connectionist model of English past tense and plural morphology. Cogn. Sci. 1999, 23, 463–490, doi:10.1207/s15516709cog2304_4.
[25]  Benjamin, L.T. Structuralism and functionalism. In A History of Psychology; Benjamin, L.T., Ed.; Blackwell Publishing: Malden, MA, USA, 2009; pp. 123–127.
[26]  Zhang, X.; Mathews, R.; Sun, R. Modeling meta-cognition in a cognitive architecture. Cogn. Syst. Res. 2006, 7, 327–338, doi:10.1016/j.cogsys.2005.09.001.
[27]  Heyd, J. Mill and Comte on psychology. J. Hist. Behav. Sci. 1989, 25, 125–138, doi:10.1002/1520-6696(198904)25:2<125::AID-JHBS2300250203>3.0.CO;2-U.
[28]  Surprenant, A.M.; Neath, I. T.V. Moore’s (1939) Cognitive Psychology. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1997, 4, 342–349, doi:10.3758/BF03210791.
[29]  Spelke, E.; Hirst, W.; Neisser, U. Skills of divided attention. Cognition 1976, 4, 215–230, doi:10.1016/0010-0277(76)90018-4.
[30]  Houghton, G.; Tipper, S.P. A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. In Inhibitory Mechanisms in Attention, Memory and Language; Dagenbach, D., Carr, T., Eds.; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 1994; pp. 53–112.
[31]  Neill, W.T.; Valdes, L.A.; Terry, K. M. Selective attention and the inhibitory control of cognition. In Interference and Inhibition in Cognition; Dempster, F.N., Brainerd, C.J., Eds.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1995; pp. 207–261.
[32]  Beck, A.T. Depression: Causes and Treatment; University of Pennsylvania Press: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 1967.
[33]  Gwilliam, P.; Wells, A.; Cartwright-Hatton, S. Does meta–cognition or responsibility predict obsessive-compulsive symptoms: A test of the metacognitive model. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 2004, 11, 137–144, doi:10.1002/cpp.402.
[34]  Rosenthal, D.M. Consciousness, Content, and metacognitive judgment. Consc. Cogn. 2000, 9, 203–214, doi:10.1006/ccog.2000.0437.
[35]  Nisbett, R.E.; Wilson, T.D. Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychol. Rev. 1977, 84, 231–259, doi:10.1037/0033-295X.84.3.231.
[36]  Wells, A. Meta-Cognition and Worry: A Cognitive Model of Generalized Anxiety Disorder, Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 1995, 23, 301–320, doi:10.1017/S1352465800015897.
[37]  Reynolds, S.; Girling, E.; Coker, S.; Eastwood, L. The effect of mental health problems on children’s ability to discriminate amongst thoughts, feelings and behaviours. Cogn. Ther. Res. 2006, 30, 599–607, doi:10.1007/s10608-006-9037-6.
[38]  Eberth, J.; Sedlmeier, P. The effects of mindfulness meditation: A meta-Analysis. Mindfulness 2012, 3, 174–189, doi:10.1007/s12671-012-0101-x.
[39]  Ritschel, L.A.; Cheavens, J.S.; Nelson, J. Dialectical behavior therapy in an intensive outpatient program with a mixed-diagnostic sample. J. Clin. Psychol. 2012, 68, 221–235, doi:10.1002/jclp.20863.
[40]  Robinson, D.N. Toward a Science of Human Nature; Columbia University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1982.
[41]  Neisser, U. Cognitive Psychology; Appleton: New York, NY, USA, 1967.

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133