全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...
PLOS ONE  2012 

Dating the Origin of Language Using Phonemic Diversity

DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0035289

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Language is a key adaptation of our species, yet we do not know when it evolved. Here, we use data on language phonemic diversity to estimate a minimum date for the origin of language. We take advantage of the fact that phonemic diversity evolves slowly and use it as a clock to calculate how long the oldest African languages would have to have been around in order to accumulate the number of phonemes they possess today. We use a natural experiment, the colonization of Southeast Asia and Andaman Islands, to estimate the rate at which phonemic diversity increases through time. Using this rate, we estimate that present-day languages date back to the Middle Stone Age in Africa. Our analysis is consistent with the archaeological evidence suggesting that complex human behavior evolved during the Middle Stone Age in Africa, and does not support the view that language is a recent adaptation that has sparked the dispersal of humans out of Africa. While some of our assumptions require testing and our results rely at present on a single case-study, our analysis constitutes the first estimate of when language evolved that is directly based on linguistic data.

References

[1]  Klein RG (2009) The human career. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 989 pp.
[2]  Tattersall I (2009) Human origins: Out of Africa. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106: 16018–16021.
[3]  Powell BB (2009) Writing: Theory and history of the technology of civilization. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.
[4]  Fitch W (2000) The evolution of speech: a comparative review. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4: 258–267.
[5]  Lieberman DE (2008) Speculations about the selective basis for modern human craniofacial form. Evolutionary Anthropology 17: 55–68.
[6]  Lieberman P (2007) The evolution of human speech: Its anatomical and neural bases. Current Anthropology 48: 39–66.
[7]  Houghton P (1993) Neandertal supralaryngeal vocal tract. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 90: 139–46.
[8]  Campbell L (2000) Time perspective in linguistics. Renfrew C, McMahon A, Trask L, editors, Time Depth in Historical Linguistics, Volume 1, Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, chapter 1. pp. 3–31.
[9]  Enard W, Przeworski M, Fisher SE, Lai CSL, Wiebe V, et al. (2002) Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language. Nature 418: 869–72.
[10]  Fisher SE, Marcus GF (2006) The eloquent ape: genes, brains and the evolution of language. Nature Reviews Genetics 7: 9–20.
[11]  Fisher SE, Scharff C (2009) FOXP2 as a molecular window into speech and language. Trends in Genetics 25: 166–77.
[12]  Zhang J, Webb DM, Podlaha O (2002) Accelerated protein evolution and origins of human-specific features: Foxp2 as an example. Genetics 162: 1825–35.
[13]  Lieberman P (2006) The FOXP2 gene, human cognition and language. International congress Series 1296: 115–126.
[14]  Coop G, Bullaughey K, Luca F, Przeworski M (2008) The timing of selection at the human FOXP2 gene. Molecular biology and evolution 25: 1257–9.
[15]  Krause J, Lalueza-Fox C, Orlando L, Enard W, Green RE, et al. (2007) The derived FOXP2 variant of modern humans was shared with Neandertals. Current Biology 17: 1908–12.
[16]  Atkinson QD (2011) Phonemic diversity supports a serial founder effect model of language expansion from Africa. Science 332: 346–349.
[17]  Mellars P (2006) Why did modern human populations disperse from Africa ca. 60,000 years ago? A new model. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103: 9381–9387.
[18]  Li JZ, Absher DM, Tang H, Southwick AM, Casto AM, et al. (2008) Worldwide human relationships inferred from genome-wide patterns of variation. Science (New York, NY) 319: 1100–4.
[19]  Prugnolle F, Manica A, Balloux F (2005) Geography predicts neutral genetic diversity of human populations. Current Biology 15: R159–R160.
[20]  Ramachandran S, Deshpande O, Roseman CC, Rosenberg NA, Feldman MW, et al. (2005) Support from the relationship of genetic and geographic distance in human populations for a serial founder effect originating in Africa. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102: 15942–7.
[21]  Betti L, Balloux F, Amos W, Hanihara T, Manica A (2009) Distance from Africa, not climate, explains within-population phenotypic diversity in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 276: 809–814.
[22]  Von Cramon-Taubadel N, Lycett SJ (2008) Brief communication: human cranial variation fits iterative founder effect model with African origin. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 136: 108–113.
[23]  Manica A, Amos W, Balloux F, Hanihara T (2007) The effect of ancient population bottlenecks on human phenotypic variation. Nature 448: 346–348.
[24]  Maddieson I (1984) Patterns of sounds. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
[25]  Maddieson I, Precoda K (1990) Updating UPSID. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 74: 104–111.
[26]  Henrich J (2004) Demography and cultural evolution: How adaptive cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses: The Tasmanian case. American Antiquity 69: 197–214.
[27]  Powell A, Shennan S, Thomas MG (2009) Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science 324: 1298–1301.
[28]  Shennan S (2001) Demography and cultural innovation: a model and its implications for the emergence of modern human culture. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 11: 5–16.
[29]  Hay J, Bauer L (2007) Phoneme inventory size and population size. Language 83: 388–400.
[30]  Croft W (2000) Explaining language change: an evolutionary approach. Oxford, UK: Oxford university Press.
[31]  Labov W (1994) Principles of linguistic change: Internal factors. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
[32]  Labov W (2001) Principles of linguistic change: Social factors. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
[33]  Labov W (2010) Principles of linguistic change: Cognitive and cultural factors.
[34]  Thomason SG, Kaufman T (1988) Language contact, creolization, and genetic linguistics. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
[35]  Yang CD (2003) Knowledge and learning in natural language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
[36]  Trudgill P (2004) Linguistic and social typology: The Austronesian migrations and phoneme inventories. Linguistic Typology 8: 305–320.
[37]  Swadesh M (1952) Lexicostatistic dating of prehistoric ethnic contacts. Proceedings American Philosophical Society 96: 452–463.
[38]  Swadesh M (1955) Towards greater accuracy in lexicostatistic dating. International Journal of American Linguistics 21: 121–137.
[39]  Lees RB (1953) The basis of glottochronology. Language 29: 113–127.
[40]  Milroy J, Milroy L (1985) Linguistic change, social network and speaker innovation. Journal of Linguistics 21: 339–384.
[41]  Nettle D (1999) Is the rate of linguistic change constant? Lingua 108: 119–136.
[42]  Pagel M, Atkinson QD, Meade A (2007) Frequency of word-use predicts rates of lexical evolution throughout Indo-European history. Nature 449: 717–20.
[43]  Bergsland K, Vogt H (1962) On the validity of glottochronology. Current Anthropology 3: 115.
[44]  Blust R, Renfrew C, McMahon A, Trask RL (2001) Why lexicostatistics doesn’t work: the “universal constant” hypothesis and the Austronesian languages. Renfrew C, McMahon A, Trask L, editors, Time depth in historical linguistics, McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, volume 2, chapter 13. pp. 311–331.
[45]  Gray RD, Atkinson QD (2003) Language-tree divergence times support the Anatolian theory of Indo-European origin. Nature 426: 435–439.
[46]  Gray RD, Drummond AJ, Greenhill SJ (2009) Language phylogenies reveal expansion pulses and pauses in Pacific settlement. Science 323: 479–483.
[47]  Gray RD, Atkinson QD, Greenhill SJ (2011) Language evolution and human history: what a difference a date makes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 366: 1090–1100.
[48]  Bybee J (2011) How plausible is the hypothesis that population size and dispersal are related to phoneme inventory size? Introducing and commenting on a debate. Linguistic Typology 15: 147–153.
[49]  Trudgill P (2011) Social structure and phoneme inventories. Linguistic Typology 15: 155–160.
[50]  Donohue M, Nichols J (2011) Does phoneme inventory size correlate with population size? Linguistic Typology 15: 161–170.
[51]  Dahl O (2011) Are small languages more or less complex than big ones? Linguistic Typology 15: 171–175.
[52]  Wichmann , Rama T, Holman , Eric W (2011) Phonological diversity, word length, and population sizes across languages: The ASJP evidence. Linguistic Typology 15: 177–197.
[53]  Sproat R (2011) Phonemic diversity and the out-of-Africa theory. Linguistic Typology 15: 199–206.
[54]  Bowern C (2011) Out of Africa? The logic of phoneme inventories and founder effects. Linguistic Typology 15: 207–216.
[55]  Pericliev V (2011) On phonemic diversity and the origin of language in Africa. Linguistic Typology 15: 217–221.
[56]  Ringe D (2011) A pilot study for an investigation into Atkinson’s hypothesis. Linguistic Typology 15: 223–231.
[57]  Rice K (2011) Athabaskan languages and serial founder effects. Linguistic Typology 15: 233–250.
[58]  Ross B, Donahue M (2011) The many origins of diversity and complexity in phonology. Linguistic Typology 15: 251–265.
[59]  Maddieson I, Bhattacharya T, Smith DE, Croft W (2011) Geographical distribution of phonological complexity. Linguistic Typology 15: 267–279.
[60]  Jaeger TF, Graff P, Croft W, Pontillo D (2011) Mixed e_ect models for genetic and areal dependencies in linguistic typology. 2011 15: 281–319.
[61]  Cysouw M, Dediu D, Moran S (2012) Comment on “Phonemic diversity supports a serial founder effect model of language expansion from Africa”. Science 335: 657.
[62]  Wang C, Ding Q, Tao H, Li H (2012) Comment on “Phonemic Diversity Supports a Serial Founder Effect Model”. Science 335: 657.
[63]  Tuyl RV, Pereltsvaig A (2012) Comment on “Phonemic Diversit’y Supports a Serial Founder Effect Model”. Science 335: 657.
[64]  Atkinson QD (2011) Linking spatial patterns of language variation to ancient demography and population migrations. Linguistic Typology 15: 321–332.
[65]  Atkinson QD (2012) Response to Comments on \Phonemic Diversity Supports a Serial Founder Effect Model of Expansion from Africa". Science 335: 657.
[66]  Lightfoot D (2006) How new languages emerge. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 199 pp.
[67]  Nettle D (1998) Explaining global patterns of language diversity. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 17: 354–374.
[68]  Forster P, Matsumura S (2006) Did early humans go north or south? Science 308: 965–966.
[69]  Macaulay V, Hill C, Achilli A, Rengo C, Clarke D, et al. (2005) Single, rapid coastal settlement of Asia revealed by analysis of complete mitochondrial genomes. Science 308: 1034–6.
[70]  Thangaraj K, Chaubey G, Kivisild T, Reddy AG, Singh VK, et al. (2005) Reconstructing the origin of Andaman Islanders. Science 308: 996.
[71]  Palanichamy MG, Sun C, Agrawal S, Bandelt HJ, Kong QP, et al. (2004) Phylogeny of mitochondrial DNA macrohaplogroup N in India, based on complete sequencing: implications for the peopling of South Asia. American Journal of Human Genetics 75: 966–78.
[72]  Endicott P, Gilbert MTP, Stringer C, Lalueza-Fox C, Willerslev E, et al. (2003) The genetic origins of the Andaman Islanders. American Journal of Human Genetics 72: 178–84.
[73]  Barker G (2005) The archaeology of foraging and farming at Niah Cave, Sarawak. Asian Perspectives 44: 90–106.
[74]  O’Connell JF, Allen J (2004) Dating the colonization of Sahul (Pleistocene Australia-New Guinea): a review of recent research. Journal of Archaeological Science 31: 835–853.
[75]  Atkinson QD, Gray RD, Drummond AJ (2008) mtDNA variation predicts population size in humans and reveals a major Southern Asian chapter in human prehistory. Molecular biology and evolution 25: 468–74.
[76]  Ericksen P, Beckerman S (1975) Population determinants in the Andaman Islands. Mankind 10: 105–107.
[77]  Thangaraj K, Singh L, Reddy AG, Rao VR, Sehgal SC, et al. (2003) Genetic affinities of the Andaman Islanders, a vanishing human population. Current Biology 13: 86–93.
[78]  Greenberg JH (1971) The Indo-Pacific hypothesis. Sebeok TA, editor, Current Trends in Linguistics vol 8: Linguistics in Oceania, Mouton de Gruyter, volume 8. pp. 809–871.
[79]  Wurm SA (1971) Classifications of Australian languages, including Tasmanian. Sebeok TA, editor, Current trends in linguistics, vol. 8: Linguistics in Oceania, Mouton de Gruyter. pp. 721–778.
[80]  Wurm SA, McElhanan K (1975) New Guinea area languages and language study, vol 1: Papuan languages and the New Guinea linguistic scene. Canberra: Australian National University.
[81]  Ruhlen M (1987) A guide to the world’s languages. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
[82]  Dryer MS, Haspelmath M (2011) The World Atlas of Language Structures online. Munich: Max Planck Digital Library.
[83]  Güldemann T, Stoneking M (2008) A historical appraisal of clicks: A linguistic and genetic population perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology 37: 93–109.
[84]  Greenberg JH (1963) The languages of Africa. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
[85]  Tishkoff SA, Gonder MK, Henn BM, Mortensen H, Knight A, et al. (2007) History of clickspeaking populations of Africa inferred from mtDNA and Y chromosome genetic variation. Molecular biology and evolution 24: 2180–95.
[86]  Knight A, Underhill PA, Mortensen HM, Zhivotovsky LA, Lin AA, et al. (2003) African Y chromosome and mtDNA divergence provides insight into the history of click languages. Current Biology 13: 464–473.
[87]  Semino O, Santachiara-Benerecetti AS, Falaschi F, Cavalli-Sforza LL, Underhill PA (2002) Ethiopians and Khoisan share the deepest clades of the human Y-chromosome phylogeny. American Journal of Human G’enetics 70: 265–8.
[88]  Chen YS, Olckers A, Schurr TG, Kogelnik AM, Huoponen K, et al. (2000) mtDNA variation in the South African Kung and Khwe-and their genetic relationships to other African populations. American journal of human genetics 66: 1362–83.
[89]  Watson E, Bauer K, Aman R, Weiss G, von Haeseler A, et al. (1996) mtDNA sequence diversity in Africa. American Journal of Human Genetics 59: 437–44.
[90]  Sands B, Güldemann T (2009) What click languages can and can’t tell us about language origins. Botha RP, Knight C, editors, The cradle of language (studies in the evolution of language), USA: Oxford University Press, chapter 11. pp. 204–218.
[91]  Diamond J (1991) The rise and fall of the third chimpanzee. Hutchinson Radius (Vintage Edition 1992), 360 pp.
[92]  Brown KS, Marean CW, Herries AIR, Jacobs Z, Tribolo C, et al. (2009) Fire as an engineering tool of early modern humans. Science 325: 859–62.
[93]  D’Errico F, Henshilwood C, Vanhaeren M, Van Niekerk K (2005) Nassarius kraussianus shell beads from Blombos Cave: evidence for symbolic behaviour in the Middle Stone Age. Journal of Human Evolution 48: 3–24.
[94]  D’Errico F, Henshilwood CS (2007) Additional evidence for bone technology in the southern African Middle Stone Age. Journal of Human Evolution 52: 142–163.
[95]  Foley R, Lahr MM (2003) On stony ground: Lithic technology, human evolution, and the emergence of culture. Evolutionary Anthropology 12: 109–122.
[96]  Henshilwood CS, Marean CW (2003) The origin of modern human behavior: Critique of the models and their test implications. Current Anthropology 44: 627–651.
[97]  Marean CA, Assefa Z (2005) The Middle and Upper Pleistocene African record for the biological and behavioral origins of modern humans. Stahl A, editor, African archaeology: A critical introduction, Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing, chapter 4. pp. 93–129.
[98]  Marean CW, Bar-Matthews M, Bernatchez J, Fisher E, Goldberg P, et al. (2007) Early human use of marine resources and pigment in South Africa during the Middle Pleistocene. Nature 449: 905–908.
[99]  Marean CW (2011) Coastal South Africa and the coevolution of the modern human lineage and the Coastal adaptation. Bicho NF, Haws JA, Davis LG, editors, Trekking the shore: Changing coastlines and the antiquity of coastal settlement, New York, NY: Springer New York, Interdisciplinary Contributions to Archaeology. pp. 421–440. doi:10.1007/978–1-4419–8219–3.
[100]  Mcbrearty S, Brooks AS (2000) The revolution that wasn’t: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. Journal of Human Evolution 39: 453–563.
[101]  Clark JD, Beyene Y, WoldeGabriel G, Hart WK, Renne PR, et al. (2003) Stratigraphic, chronological and behavioural contexts of Pleistocene Homo sapiens from Middle Awash, Ethiopia. Nature 423: 747–752.
[102]  McDougall I, Brown FH, Fleagle JG (2005) Stratigraphic placement and age of modern humans from Kibish, Ethiopia. Nature 433: 733–736.
[103]  Smith TM, Ta_oreau P, Reid DJ, Grün R, Eggins S, et al. (2007) Earliest evidence of modern human life history in North African early Homo sapiens. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104: 6128–6133.
[104]  White TD, Asfaw B, DeGusta D, Gilbert H, Richards GD, et al. (2003) Pleistocene Homo sapiens from Middle Awash, Ethiopia. Nature 423: 742–747.
[105]  Behar DM, Villems R, Soodyall H, Blue-Smith J, Pereira L, et al. (2008) The dawn of human matrilineal diversity. Journal of Human Genetics 82: 1130–1140.
[106]  Fagundes NJR, Ray N, Beaumont M, Neuenschwander S, Salzano FM, et al. (2007) Statistical evaluation of alternative models of human evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104: 17614–17619.
[107]  Ingman M, Kaessmann H, P??bo S, Gyllensten U (2000) Mitochondrial genome variation and the origin of modern humans. Nature 408: 708–713.
[108]  Gonder MK, Mortensen HM, Reed FA, De Sousa A, Tishkoff SA (2007) Whole-mtDNA genome sequence analysis of ancient African lineages. Molecular Biology and Evolution 24: 757–768.

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133