全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...
PLOS ONE  2012 

Neuroimaging Evidence for Processes Underlying Repetition of Ignored Stimuli

DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036089

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Prolonged response times are observed with targets having been presented as distractors immediately before, called negative priming effect. Among others, inhibitory and retrieval processes have been suggested underlying this behavioral effect. As those processes would involve different neural activation patterns, a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study including 28 subjects was conducted. Two tasks were used to investigate stimulus repetition effects. One task focused on target location, the other on target identity. Both tasks are known to elicit the expected response time effects. However, there is less agreement about the relationship of those tasks with the explanatory accounts under consideration. Based on within-subject comparisons we found clear differences between the experimental repetition conditions and the neutral control condition on neural level for both tasks. Hemodynamic fronto-striatal activation patterns occurred for the location-based task favoring the selective inhibition account. Hippocampal activation found for the identity-based task suggests an assignment to the retrieval account; however, this task lacked a behavioral effect.

References

[1]  Egner T, Hirsch J (2005) Where memory meets attention: Neural substrates of negative priming. J Cogn Neurosci 17: 1774–1784.
[2]  Tipper SP (1985) The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. Q J Exp Psychol A37: 571–590.
[3]  Gibbons H (2006) An event-related potential investigation of varieties of negative priming. Psychophysiology 20: 170–185.
[4]  May CP, Kane MJ, Hasher , L (1995) Determinants of negative priming. Psychol Bull 118: 35–54.
[5]  Christie J, Klein RM (2001) Negative priming for spatial location? Can J Exp Psychol 55: 24–38.
[6]  Tipper SP (1991) Less attentional selectivity as a result of declining inhibition in older adults. Bull Psychon Soc 29: 45–47.
[7]  Fischer R, Hagendorf , H (2006) The control of visual attention and its influence on prioritized processing in a location negative priming paradigm. Psychol Res 70: 317–335.
[8]  Mayr S, Buchner A (2007) Negative priming as a memory phenomenon: A review of 20 years of negative priming research. J Psychol 215: 35–51.
[9]  Tipper SP (2001) Does negative priming reflect inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integration of conflicting views. Q J Exp Psychol A54: 321–343.
[10]  Houghton G, Tipper SP (1994) A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. In: Dagenbach D, Carr T, editors. Inhibitory mechanisms in attention memory and language. San Diego: Academic Press. pp. 53–112.
[11]  Neill WT, Valdes LA (1992) Persistence of negative priming: Steady state or decay? J Exp Psychol Learn 18: 565–576.
[12]  Neill WT, Valdes LA, Terry KM, Gorfein DS (1992) Persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. J Exp Psychol Learn 18: 993–1000.
[13]  Buckolz E, Boulougouris A, Khan M (2002) Influence of probe-trial selection on the location negative priming effect. Can J Exp Psychol 56: 273–282.
[14]  Guy S, Buckolz E (2007) The locus and modulation of the location negative priming effect. Psychol Res 71: 178–191.
[15]  Vink M, Kahn RS, Raemaekers M, Ramsey NF (2005) Perceptual bias following visual target selection. Neuroimage 25: 1168–1174.
[16]  Frings C, Groh-Bordin C (2007) Electrophysiological correlates of visual identity negative priming. Brain Res 1176: 82–91.
[17]  Casey BJ, Durston S, Fossella JA (2001) Evidence for a mechanistic model of cognitive control. Clin Neurosci Res 1: 267–282.
[18]  Aron AR, Monsell S, Sahakian BJ, Robins TW (2004) A componential analysis of task-switching deficits associated with lesions of left and right frontal cortex. Brain 127. pp. 1561–1573.
[19]  Konishi S, Nakajima K, Uchida I, Kikyo H, Kameyama M, et al. (1999) Common inhibitory mechanism in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by event-related fMRI. Brain 122: 981–91.
[20]  Swainson R, Cunnington R, Jackson GM, Rorden C, Peters AM, et al. (2003) Cognitive control mechanisms revealed by ERP and fMRI: evidence from repeated task-switching. J Cogn Neurosci, 15: 785–799.
[21]  Michael GA, Garcia S, Fernandez D, Sellal F, Boucart M (2006) The ventral premotor cortex (vPM) and resistance to interference. Behav Neurosci 120: 447–62.
[22]  Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW , Cho RY, Stenger VA, et al. (2004) Anterior cingulated conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science 303: 1023–1026.
[23]  Ullsperger M, von Cramon DY (2001) Subprocesses of performance monitoring: A dissociation of error processing and response competition revealed by event-related fMRI and ERPs. Neuroimage 14: 1387–1401.
[24]  Carter CS, Macdonald AM, Botvinick M, Ross LL, Stenger VA, et al. (2000) Parsing executive processes: Strategic vs. evaluative functions of the anterior cingulate cortex. PNAS 97: 1944–1948.
[25]  Casey BJ, Thomas KM, Welsh TF, Badgaiyan RD, Eccard CH, et al. (2000) Dissociation of response conflict, attentional selection, and expectancy with functional magnetic resonance imaging. PNAS 97: 8728–8733.
[26]  Botvinick MM, Cohen JD, Carter CS (2004) Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: An update. Trends Cogn Sci 8: 539–546.
[27]  Logan GD (1988) Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychol Rev 95: 492–527.
[28]  Neill WT (1997) Episodic retrieval in negative priming and repetition priming. J Exp Psychol Learn 23: 1291–1305.
[29]  Rothermund K, Wentura D, De Houwer J (2005) Retrieval of incidental stimulus-response associations as a source of negative priming. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 31: 482–495.
[30]  Eldridge LL, Engel SA, Zeineh MM, Bookheimer SY, Knowlton BJ (2005) A dissociation of encoding and retrieval processes in the human hippocampus. J Neurosci 25: 3280–3286.
[31]  Zeineh MM, Engel SA, Thompson PM, Bookheimer SY (2003) Dynamics of the hippocampus during encoding and retrieval of face-name pairs. Science 299: 577–580.
[32]  Eldridge LL, Knowlton BJ, Furmanski CS, Bookheimer SY, Engel , SA (2000) Remembering episodes: a selective role for the hippocampus during retrieval. Nat Neurosci 3: 1149–52.
[33]  Yonelinas AP, Otten LJ, Shaw KN, Rugg MD (2005) Separating the brain regions involved in recollection and familiarity in recognition memory. J Neurosci 25: 3002–3008.
[34]  Davachi L (2006) Item, context and relational episodic encoding in humans. Curr Opin Neurobiol 16: 693–700.
[35]  Diana RA, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C (2007) Imaging recollection and familiarity in the medial temporal lobe: a three-component model. Trends Cogn Sci 11: 379–386.
[36]  Neill WT (2007) Mechanisms of transfer-inappropriate processing. In: Gorfein DS, MacLeod CM, editors. Inhibition in cognition. Washington DC: American Psychological Association. pp. 63–79.
[37]  Frings C, Spence C (2011) Increased perceptual and conceptual processing difficulty makes the immeasurable measurable: negative priming in the absence of probe distractors. J Exp Psychol Human 37: 72–84.
[38]  Amieva HA, Lafont S, Auriacombe S, Le Carret N, Dartigues J-F, et al. (2002) Inhibitory breakdown and dementia of the Alzheimer Type: A general phenomenon? J Clin Exp Neuropsyc 24: 503–516.
[39]  Sullivan MP, Faust ME, Balota DA (1995) Identity negative priming in older adults and individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer Type. Neuropsychology 9: 537–555.
[40]  Troche SJ, Trenkwalder C, Morelli-Canelo M, Gibbons H, Rammsayer TH (2006) Unimpaired negative but enhanced positive priming in Parkinson’s disease: Evidence from an identity and a location priming task. Neuropsychologia 44: 1811–1821.
[41]  Troche SJ, Trenkwalder C, Morelli-Canelo M, Gibbons H, Rammsayer TH (2009) To the influence of general slowing and medication on identity- and location-based priming effects in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Neuropsychol 3: 147–168.
[42]  Tipper SP, Bourque TA, Anderson SH, Brehaut JC (1989) Mechanisms of attention: a developmental study. J Exp Psychol 48: 353–378.
[43]  Tipper SP, McLaren , J (1990) Evidence for efficient visual selectivity in children. In: Enns JT, editor. The development of attention: Research and theory. North-Holland: Elsevier Science Publishers. pp. 197–210.
[44]  Milliken B, Tipper SP, Houghton G, Lupiá?ez J (2000) Attending, ignoring, and repetition: on the relation between negative priming and inhibition of return. Percept Psychophys 62: 1280–1296.
[45]  Henson R (2005) What can functional neuroimaging tell the experimental psychologist? Q J Exp Psychol-A 58: 193–233.
[46]  Stolz JA, Neely JH (2001) Taking a bright view of negative priming in the light of dim stimuli: further evidence for memory confusion during episodic retrieval. Can J Exp Psychol 55: 219–230.
[47]  Kathmann N, Bogdahn B, Endrass T (2006) Event-related brain potential variations during location and identity negative priming. Neurosci Lett 394: 53–56.
[48]  Wright CI, Keuthen NJ, Savage CR, Marits B, Williams D, et al. (2006) Brain correlates of negative and positive visuospatial priming in adults. Neuroimage 30: 983–991.
[49]  Krueger F, Fischer R, Heinecke A, Hagendorf H (2007) An fMRI investigation into the neural mechanisms of spatial attentional selection in a location-based negative priming task. Brain Res 1174: 110–119.
[50]  Steel C, Haworth EJ, Peters E, Hemsley DR, Sharma T, et al. (2001) Neuroimaging correlates of negative priming. Neuroreport 12: 3619–3624.
[51]  Rugg MD, Henson RN, Robb WG (2003) Neural correlates of retrieval processing in the prefrontal cortex during recognition and exclusion tasks. Neuropsychologia 41: 40–52.
[52]  Henson RN, Rugg MD, Shallice T, Dolan RJ (2000) Confidence in recognition memory for words: Dissociating right prefrontal roles in episodic retrieval. J Cogn Neurosci 12: 913–923.
[53]  Stadler MA, Hogan ME (1996) Varieties of positive and negative priming. Psychon B Rev 3: 87–90.
[54]  Gibbons H, Rammsayer TH, Stahl J (2006) Multiple sources of positive- and negative-priming effects: An event-related potential study. Mem Cognition 34: 172–186.
[55]  Mayr S, Niedeggen M, Buchner A, Pietrowsky R (2003) ERP correlates of auditory negative priming. Cognition 90: 11–21.
[56]  Gibbons H, Frings C (2010) Flanker negative priming from spatially unpredictable primes: An ERP study. Int J Psycholphysiol 75: 339–348.
[57]  Maldjian JA, Laurienti PJ, Kraft RA, Burdette JH (2003) An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI datasets. Neuroimage 19: 1233–1239.
[58]  Lancaster JL, Summerlin JL, Rainey L, Freitas CS, Fox PT (1997) The Talairach Daemon, a database server for Talairach atlas labels. Neuroimage 5: 633.
[59]  Lancaster JL, Woldorff MG, Parsons LM, Liotti M, Freitas CS, et al. (2000) Automated Talairach atlas labels for functional brain mapping. Hum Brain Ma 10: 120–131.
[60]  Talairach J, Tournoux P (1988) Co-planar stereotactic atlas of the human brain: 3-dimensional proportional system: an approach to cerebral imaging. Stuttgart: Thieme.
[61]  Houghton G, Tipper SP (1996) Inhibitory mechanisms of neural and cognitive control: applications to selective attention and sequential action. Brain Cognition 30: 20–43.
[62]  Cropley VL, Fujita M, Innis RB, Nathan PJ (2006) Molecular imaging of the dopaminergic system and its association with human cognitive function. Biol Psychiat 59: 898–907.
[63]  Robbins TW (2007) Shifting and stopping: fronto-striatal substrates, neurochemical modulation and clinical implications. Philos T Roy Soc B 362: 917–932.
[64]  Cools R, Ivry RB, D'Esposito M (2006) The human striatum is necessary for responding to changes in stimulus relevance. J Cognitive Neurosci 18: 1973–83.

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133