全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...
PLOS ONE  2012 

Neuronal Mechanisms of Voice Control Are Affected by Implicit Expectancy of Externally Triggered Perturbations in Auditory Feedback

DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0041216

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Accurate vocal production relies on several factors including sensory feedback and the ability to predict future challenges to the control processes. Repetitive patterns of perturbations in sensory feedback by themselves elicit implicit expectations in the vocal control system regarding the timing, quality and direction of perturbations. In the present study, the predictability of voice pitch-shifted auditory feedback was experimentally manipulated. A block of trials where all pitch-shift stimuli were upward, and therefore predictable was contrasted against an unpredictable block of trials in which the stimulus direction was randomized between upward and downward pitch-shifts. It was found that predictable perturbations in voice auditory feedback led to a reduction in the proportion of compensatory vocal responses, which might be indicative of a reduction in vocal control. The predictable perturbations also led to a reduction in the magnitude of the N1 component of cortical Event Related Potentials (ERP) that was associated with the reflexive compensations to the perturbations. We hypothesize that formation of expectancy in our study is accompanied by involuntary allocation of attentional resources occurring as a result of habituation or learning, that in turn trigger limited and controlled exploration-related motor variability in the vocal control system.

References

[1]  Brunia CH (1999) Neural aspects of anticipatory behavior. Acta Psychol (Amst) 101: 213–242.
[2]  Bubic A, von Cramon DY, Schubotz RI (2010) Prediction, cognition and the brain. Front Hum Neurosci 4: 25.
[3]  Antle MC, Silver R (2009) Neural basis of timing and anticipatory behaviors. Eur J Neurosci 30: 1643–1649.
[4]  Pavlov IP (1955) Selected works. Moscow,: Foreign Languages Pub. House. 653 p.
[5]  Dayan P, Niv Y (2008) Reinforcement learning: the good, the bad and the ugly. Curr Opin Neurobiol 18: 185–196.
[6]  Bendor D, Wang X (2010) Neural coding of periodicity in marmoset auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol 103: 1809–1822.
[7]  Kotz SA, Schwartze M, Schmidt-Kassow M (2009) Non-motor basal ganglia functions: a review and proposal for a model of sensory predictability in auditory language perception. Cortex 45: 982–990.
[8]  Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z, Jordan MI (1995) An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science 269: 1880–1882.
[9]  Wolpert DM, Flanagan JR (2001) Motor prediction. Curr Biol 11: R729–732.
[10]  Wolpert DM, Miall RC (1996) Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control. Neural Netw 9: 1265–1279.
[11]  Franklin DW, Wolpert DM (2011) Computational mechanisms of sensorimotor control. Neuron 72: 425–442.
[12]  Bendixen A, Schroger E, Winkler I (2009) I heard that coming: event-related potential evidence for stimulus-driven prediction in the auditory system. J Neurosci 29: 8447–8451.
[13]  Winkler I, Denham SL, Nelken I (2009) Modeling the auditory scene: predictive regularity representations and perceptual objects. Trends Cogn Sci 13: 532–540.
[14]  Wright BA, Fitzgerald MB (2004) The time course of attention in a simple auditory detection task. Percept Psychophys 66: 508–516.
[15]  Burnett TA, Freedland MB, Larson CR, Hain TC (1998) Voice F0 responses to manipulations in pitch feedback. J Acoust Soc Am 103: 3153–3161.
[16]  Kawahara H (1994) Interactions between speech production and perception under auditory feedback perturbations on fundamental frequencies. Journal of the Acoustical Society of Japan 15: 201–202.
[17]  Behroozmand R, Karvelis L, Liu H, Larson CR (2009) Vocalization-induced enhancement of the auditory cortex responsiveness during voice F0 feedback perturbation. Clin Neurophysiol 120: 1303–1312.
[18]  Behroozmand R, Korzyukov O, Larson CR (2011) Effects of voice harmonic complexity on ERP responses to pitch-shifted auditory feedback. Clin Neurophysiol 122: 2408–2417.
[19]  Korzyukov O, Karvelis L, Behroozmand R, Larson CR (2012) ERP correlates of auditory processing during automatic correction of unexpected perturbations in voice auditory feedback. Int J Psychophysiol 83: 71–78.
[20]  Liu H, Meshman M, Behroozmand R, Larson CR (2011) Differential effects of perturbation direction and magnitude on the neural processing of voice pitch feedback. Clin Neurophysiol 122: 951–957.
[21]  Burkard RF, Eggermont JJ, Don M (2007) Auditory evoked potentials : basic principles and clinical application. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. xix, 731 p., [716] p. of plates p.
[22]  Eliades SJ, Wang X (2008) Neural substrates of vocalization feedback monitoring in primate auditory cortex. Nature 453: 1102–1106.
[23]  Curio G, Neuloh G, Numminen J, Jousmaki V, Hari R (2000) Speaking modifies voice-evoked activity in the human auditory cortex. Hum Brain Mapp 9: 183–191.
[24]  Heinks-Maldonado TH, Mathalon DH, Gray M, Ford JM (2005) Fine-tuning of auditory cortex during speech production. Psychophysiology 42: 180–190.
[25]  Houde JF, Nagarajan SS, Sekihara K, Merzenich MM (2002) Modulation of the auditory cortex during speech: an MEG study. J Cogn Neurosci 14: 1125–1138.
[26]  Ford JM, Mathalon DH, Kalba S, Whitfield S, Faustman WO, et al. (2001) Cortical responsiveness during talking and listening in schizophrenia: an event-related brain potential study. Biol Psychiatry 50: 540–549.
[27]  Behroozmand R, Larson CR (2011) Error-dependent modulation of speech-induced auditory suppression for pitch-shifted voice feedback. BMC Neurosci 12: 54.
[28]  Fuster JM (2001) The prefrontal cortex–an update: time is of the essence. Neuron 30: 319–333.
[29]  Baars BJ, Franklin S (2003) How conscious experience and working memory interact. Trends Cogn Sci 7: 166–172.
[30]  Fuster JM (2004) Upper processing stages of the perception-action cycle. Trends Cogn Sci 8: 143–145.
[31]  Oostenveld R, Praamstra P (2001) The five percent electrode system for high-resolution EEG and ERP measurements. Clin Neurophysiol 112: 713–719.
[32]  Boersma P (2001) Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer. GLOT International 5: 341–345.
[33]  McCarthy G, Wood CC (1985) Scalp distributions of event-related potentials: an ambiguity associated with analysis of variance models. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 62: 203–208.
[34]  Hain TC, Burnett TA, Kiran S, Larson CR, Singh S, et al. (2000) Instructing subjects to make a voluntary response reveals the presence of two components to the audio-vocal reflex. Exp Brain Res 130: 133–141.
[35]  Todorov E, Jordan MI (2002) Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nat Neurosci 5: 1226–1235.
[36]  Mandelblat-Cerf Y, Paz R, Vaadia E (2009) Trial-to-trial variability of single cells in motor cortices is dynamically modified during visuomotor adaptation. J Neurosci 29: 15053–15062.
[37]  Chait M, Simon JZ, Poeppel D (2004) Auditory M50 and M100 responses to broadband noise: functional implications. Neuroreport 15: 2455–2458.
[38]  Murray MM, Camen C, Gonzalez Andino SL, Bovet P, Clarke S (2006) Rapid brain discrimination of sounds of objects. J Neurosci 26: 1293–1302.
[39]  Roberts TP, Ferrari P, Stufflebeam SM, Poeppel D (2000) Latency of the auditory evoked neuromagnetic field components: stimulus dependence and insights toward perception. J Clin Neurophysiol 17: 114–129.
[40]  Conley EM, Michalewski HJ, Starr A (1999) The N100 auditory cortical evoked potential indexes scanning of auditory short-term memory. Clin Neurophysiol 110: 2086–2093.
[41]  Lu ZL, Williamson SJ, Kaufman L (1992) Behavioral lifetime of human auditory sensory memory predicted by physiological measures. Science 258: 1668–1670.
[42]  Baess P, Horvath J, Jacobsen T, Schroger E (2011) Selective suppression of self-initiated sounds in an auditory stream: An ERP study. Psychophysiology 48: 1276–1283.
[43]  Martikainen MH, Kaneko K, Hari R (2005) Suppressed responses to self-triggered sounds in the human auditory cortex. Cereb Cortex 15: 299–302.
[44]  Larson CR, Sun J, Hain TC (2007) Effects of simultaneous perturbations of voice pitch and loudness feedback on voice F0 and amplitude control. J Acoust Soc Am 121: 2862–2872.
[45]  Alipour-Haghighi F, Perlman AL, Titze IR (1991) Tetanic response of the cricothyroid muscle. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol 100: 626–631.
[46]  Larson CR, Kempster GB, Kistler MK (1987) Changes in voice fundamental frequency following discharge of single motor units in cricothyroid and thyroarytenoid muscles. J Speech Hear Res 30: 552–558.
[47]  Ludlow CL, Lou G (1996) Observations on human laryngeal muscle control. In: Davis PJ, Fletcher NH, editors. pp. 201–218. San Diego: Singular.
[48]  Rodel RM, Olthoff A, Tergau F, Simonyan K, Kraemer D, et al. (2004) Human cortical motor representation of the larynx as assessed by transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). Laryngoscope 114: 918–922.
[49]  Giard MH, Perrin F, Echallier JF, Thevenet M, Froment JC, et al. (1994) Dissociation of temporal and frontal components in the human auditory N1 wave: a scalp current density and dipole model analysis. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 92: 238–252.
[50]  Rankin CH, Abrams T, Barry RJ, Bhatnagar S, Clayton DF, et al. (2009) Habituation revisited: an updated and revised description of the behavioral characteristics of habituation. Neurobiol Learn Mem 92: 135–138.
[51]  Groves PM, Thompson RF (1970) Habituation: a dual-process theory. Psychol Rev 77: 419–450.
[52]  Sokolov EN (1963) Perception and the conditioned reflex. Oxford, New York,: Pergamon Press. x, 309 p.
[53]  Siddle DA (1991) Orienting, habituation, and resource allocation: an associative analysis. Psychophysiology 28: 245–259.
[54]  Ghajar J, Ivry RB (2009) The predictive brain state: asynchrony in disorders of attention? Neuroscientist 15: 232–242.
[55]  Deroost N, Soetens E (2006) Perceptual or motor learning in SRT tasks with complex sequence structures. Psychol Res 70: 88–102.

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133