[1] | Turing AM (1950) Computing machinery and intelligence. Mind 59: 433–460.
|
[2] | McCarthy J (1979) Ascribing Mental Qualities to Machines. In: Ringle M, editor. Philosophical Perspectives in Artificial Intelligence. Harvester Press.
|
[3] | Wood G (2002) Living Dolls: A Magical History of the Quest for Mechanical Life: Faber & Faber.
|
[4] | Premack D, Woodruff G (1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioural and Brain Science 1: 515–526.
|
[5] | Saxe R, Wexler A (2005) Making sense of another mind: the role of the right temporo-parietal junction. Neuropsychologia 43: 1391–1399.
|
[6] | Frith CD, Frith U (1999) Interacting minds-a biological basis. Science 286: 1692–1695.
|
[7] | Butterworth G (1991) The ontogeny and phylogeny of joint visual attention. In: Whiten A, editor. Natural Theories of Mind: Evolution, Development and Simulation of Everyday Mindreading Oxford. Basil Blackwell. pp. 223–232.
|
[8] | Leslie AM (1994) ToMM, ToBy, and Agency: Core architecture and domain specificity. In: Hirschfeld LA, Gelman SA, editors. Mapping the mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 119–149.
|
[9] | Frith C (1996) The role of the prefrontal cortex in self-consciousness: the case of auditory hallucinations. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 351: 1505–1512.
|
[10] | Kircher TT, Leube DT (2003) Self-consciousness, self-agency, and schizophrenia. Conscious Cogn 12: 656–669.
|
[11] | Langton SR (2000) The mutual influence of gaze and head orientation in the analysis of social attention direction. Q J Exp Psychol A 53: 825–845.
|
[12] | Frith CD, Frith U (2006) The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron 50: 531–534.
|
[13] | Gallagher HL, Happe F, Brunswick N, Fletcher PC, Frith U, et al. (2000) Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks. Neuropsychologia 38: 11–21.
|
[14] | Vogeley K, Bussfeld P, Newen A, Herrmann S, Happe F, et al. (2001) Mind reading: neural mechanisms of theory of mind and self-perspective. Neuroimage 14: 170–181.
|
[15] | Brunet E, Sarfati Y, Hardy-Bayle MC, Decety J (2000) A PET investigation of the attribution of intentions with a nonverbal task. Neuroimage 11: 157–166.
|
[16] | Baron-Cohen S, Ring HA, Wheelwright S, Bullmore ET, Brammer MJ, et al. (1999) Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study. Eur J Neurosci 11: 1891–1898.
|
[17] | Castelli F, Happe F, Frith U, Frith C (2000) Movement and mind: a functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. Neuroimage 12: 314–325.
|
[18] | Rilling JK, Sanfey AG, Aronson JA, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD (2004) Opposing BOLD responses to reciprocated and unreciprocated altruism in putative reward pathways. Neuroreport 15: 2539–2543.
|
[19] | Rilling JK, Sanfey AG, Aronson JA, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD (2004) The neural correlates of theory of mind within interpersonal interactions. Neuroimage 22: 1694–1703.
|
[20] | Rilling JK, Gutman D, Zeh T, Pagnoni G, Berns G, et al. (2002) A neural basis for social cooperation. Neuron 35: 395–405.
|
[21] | Decety J, Jackson PL, Sommerville JA, Chaminade T, Meltzoff AN (2004) The neural bases of cooperation and competition: an fMRI investigation. Neuroimage 23: 744–751.
|
[22] | Gallagher HL, Jack AI, Roepstorff A, Frith CD (2002) Imaging the intentional stance in a competitive game. Neuroimage 16: 814–821.
|
[23] | McCabe K, Houser D, Ryan L, Smith V, Trouard T (2001) A functional imaging study of cooperation in two-person reciprocal exchange. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98: 11832–11835.
|
[24] | Fukui H, Murai T, Shinozaki J, Aso T, Fukuyama H, et al. (2006) The neural basis of social tactics: An fMRI study. Neuroimage 32: 913–920.
|
[25] | Gallagher HL, Frith CD (2003) Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’. Trends Cogn Sci 7: 77–83.
|
[26] | Fletcher PC, Happe F, Frith U, Baker SC, Dolan RJ, et al. (1995) Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension. Cognition 57: 109–128.
|
[27] | Amodio DM, Frith CD (2006) Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 7: 268–277.
|
[28] | Goel V, Grafman J, Sadato N, Hallett M (1995) Modeling other minds. Neuroreport 6: 1741–1746.
|
[29] | Saxe R, Kanwisher N (2003) People thinking about thinking people. The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind”. Neuroimage 19: 1835–1842.
|
[30] | Decety J, Grezes J (2006) The power of simulation: imagining one's own and other's behavior. Brain Res 1079: 4–14.
|
[31] | Pelphrey KA, Morris JP, McCarthy G (2004) Grasping the intentions of others: the perceived intentionality of an action influences activity in the superior temporal sulcus during social perception. J Cogn Neurosci 16: 1706–1716.
|
[32] | Blakemore SJ, Frith C (2003) Self-awareness and action. Curr Opin Neurobiol 13: 219–224.
|
[33] | Jackson PL, Decety J (2004) Motor cognition: a new paradigm to study self-other interactions. Curr Opin Neurobiol 14: 259–263.
|
[34] | Leube DT, Knoblich G, Erb M, Kircher TT (2003) Observing one's hand become anarchic: an fMRI study of action identification. Conscious Cogn 12: 597–608.
|
[35] | Leube DT, Knoblich G, Erb M, Grodd W, Bartels M, et al. (2003) The neural correlates of perceiving one's own movements. Neuroimage 20: 2084–2090.
|
[36] | Keysers C, Gazzola V (2006) Towards a unifying neural theory of social cognition. Prog Brain Res 156: 379–401.
|
[37] | Foerster Hv (1997) Wissen und Gewissen. Versuch einer Brücke. Frankfurt.
|
[38] | Duffy B (2003) Anthropomorphism and The Social Robot. Special Issue on Socially Interactive Robots, Robotics and Autonomous Systems 42: 3–4.
|
[39] | DiSalvo CF, Gemperle F, Forlizzi J, Kiesler SAll robots are not created equal: the design and perception of humanoid robot heads; 2002;. London, UK.
|
[40] | Epley N, Waytz A, Cacioppo JT (2007) On Seeing Human: A Three-Factor Theory of Anthropomorphism. Psychological Review 114: 864–886.
|
[41] | Jung Y, Lee KMEffects of physical embodiment on social presence of social robots; 2004. pp. 80–87.
|
[42] | Dautenhahn K, Ogdena B, Quickb T (2002) From embodied to socially embedded agents-Implications for interaction-aware robots. Cognitive Systems Research 3: 397–428.
|
[43] | Morewedge CK, Preston J, Wegner DM (2007) Timescale bias in the attribution of mind. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 93: 1–11.
|
[44] | Gockley R, Forlizzi J, Simmons RInteractions with a moody robot; 2006;. New York, USA: ACM Press. pp. 186–193.
|
[45] | Bruce A, Nourbakhsh I, Simmons R (2001) The role of expressiveness and attention in humanrobot interaction. Proc AAAI Fall Symp Emotional and Intel II: The Tangled Knot of Soc Cognition.
|
[46] | Hegel F, Spexard T, Vogt T, Horstmann G, Wrede BPlaying a different imitation game: Interaction with an Empathic Android Robot.; 2006. Proc. 2006 IEEE-RAS. pp. 56–61.
|
[47] | Duffy B, Joue GI, robot being; 2004;. Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
|
[48] | Goetz J, Kiesler S, Powers A (2003) Matching robot appearance and behavior to tasks to improve human-robot cooperation. pp. 55–60. The 12th IEEE International Workshop on Robot and Human Interactive Communication.
|
[49] | Fong TW, Nourbakhsh I, Dautenhahn K (2002) A Survey of Socially Interactive Robots: Concepts, Design, and Applications. Robotics and Autonomous Systems 42: 142–166.
|
[50] | Brickenkamp R (2002) Der Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungstest d2. G?ttingen: Hogrefe.
|
[51] | Reitan R, Wolfson D (1985) The Halstead-Reitan neuropsychological test battery: Theory and clinical interpretation. Tucson: Neuropsychology Press.
|
[52] | Lehrl S (2007) Der Mehrfachwahl- Wortschatz- Intelligenztest. G?ttingen: Hogrefe.
|
[53] | Dresher M (1961) The Mathematics of Games of Strategy: Theory and Applications. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
|
[54] | Talairach J, Tournoux P (1988) Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. Stuttgart, Germany: Thieme.
|
[55] | Dennett D (1987) The intentional stance. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
|
[56] | Cavanna AE (2007) The precuneuos and consciousness. CNS Spectr 12: 545–552.
|
[57] | Cavanna AE, Trimble MR (2006) The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 129: 564–583.
|
[58] | Decety J, Sommerville JA (2003) Shared representations between self and other: a social cognitive neuroscience view. Trends Cogn Sci 7: 527–533.
|
[59] | Mitchell JP, Neil Macrae C, Banaji MR (2005) Forming impressions of people versus inanimate objects: social-cognitive processing in the medial prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 26: 251–257.
|
[60] | Ruby P, Decety J (2001) Effect of subjective perspective taking during simulation of action: a PET investigation of agency. Nat Neurosci 4: 546–550.
|
[61] | Ghazanfar AA, Turesson HK (2008) How robots will teach us how the brain works. Nature Neuroscience 11:
|