All Title Author
Keywords Abstract


The Social Framework of Learning via Neurodidactics

DOI: 10.4236/ce.2016.715215, PP. 2175-2192

Keywords: Associative Learning, Neurodidactics, Emotion, Motivation, Memory, Socialization

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Both from a pedagogical and a psychological point of view, but also from sociology, we are aware of the fact that learning is a permanently successful process, taking into consideration that learning in the school environment does not bear significant practical differences from learning in our normal, social lives. Since the 18th century already, educational theories have been developed, accepting teaching methods focusing on learning processes, in accordance with human’s brain functions and aiming not for an individual, but a social aspect of learning, enhancing the individual’s self- efficacy in society. Neurodidactics investigates these two parameters, in an effort to introduce brain research scientific results into courses, forging at the same time the frameworks and prerequisites used to establish knowledge that was correctly structured and integrated in a context of emotional motivation. The neurodidactics’ aim is to encourage and support the management and process of learning, in a stress-free, reliable, social learning context. The following analysis of these theories should assist teachers in understanding and explaining their students’ experiences and behaviors, which should always be related to the students’ brain functions and physical-mental functions, as part of a learning group.

References

[1]  Arnold, M. B. (1960). Emotion and Personality. Psychological Aspects, Vol. 1. New York: Columbia University Press.
[2]  Atkinson, J. W. (1964). An Introduction to Motivation. New York: Litton Educational Publishing.
[3]  Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human Memory: A Proposed System and Its Control Processes. In K. W. Spence, & J. T. Spence (Eds.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory (Vol. 2, pp. 89-195). New York: Academic Press.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0079-7421(08)60422-3

[4]  Azrin, N. H., & Holz, W. C. (1966). Punishment. In W. K. Honig (Ed.), Operant Behavior: Areas of Research and Application (pp. 213-270). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
[5]  Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working Memory. In G. A. Bower (Ed.), Recent Advances in Learning and Motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47-89). New York: Academic Press.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0079-7421(08)60452-1

[6]  Bailey, D., Bruer, J., Symons, F., & Lichtman, J. (2001). Critical Thinking about Critical Periods. Baltimore, ML: Paul H. Brooks Publishing Co.
[7]  Bandura, A. (1976). Lernen am Modell. Ansatze zu einer sozial-kognitiven Lerntheorie. Stuttgart: Klett.
[8]  Bauer, J. (2009). Kleine Zellen, grobe Gefühle—wie Spiegelneurone funktionieren. Die neurobiologischen Grundlagen der “Theory of Mind”. In Hermann, U. (Ed.), Neurodidaktik. Grundlagen und Vorschlage für gehirngerechtes Lehren und Lernen (2nd ed., pp. 49-57). Weinheim & Basel: Beltz Verlag.
[9]  Brown, R., & Kulik, J. (1977). Flashbulb Memories. Cognition, 5, 73-99.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(77)90018-X

[10]  Cannon, W. B. (1927). The James-Lange Theory of Emotions: A Critical Examination and an Alternative Theory. The American Journal of Psychology, 39, 106-124.

http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1415404

[11]  Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2000). Evolutionary Psychology and the Emotions. In M. Lewis, & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of Emotions (2nd ed., pp. 91-115). New York: Guilford.
[12]  Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ Irrtum—Fühlen, Denken und das menschliche Gehirn. München: List.
[13]  Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and Cultural Differences in Facial Expressions of Emotion. In J. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 19, pp. 207-282). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
[14]  Fazio, R. H. (2001). On the Automatic Activation of Associated Evaluations. An Overview. Cognition & Emotion, 15, 115-141.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930125908

[15]  Fodor, J. (1983). The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.
[16]  Fridlund, A. J. (1991). The Sociality of Solitary Smiles: Effects of an Implicit Audience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 229-240.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.229

[17]  Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation Intentions: Strong Effects of Simple Plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493-503.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.493

[18]  Heider, F. (1975). Psychologie der interpersonalen Beziehungen. Stuttgart: Klett.
[19]  Hermann, U. (2008). Lernen—Vom Gehirn aus betrachtet. Wie schulisches Lernen verbessert werden kann: Neurowissenschaften und Padagogik auf dem gemeinsamen Weg zur Neurodida-ktik.

http://www.spektrum.de

[20]  Heckhausen, H., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1987). Thought Contents and Cognitive Functioning in Motivational versus Volitional States of Mind. Motivation & Emotion, 11, 101-120.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00992338

[21]  Hoppe, F. (1931). Erfolg und Mierfolg. Psychologie Forschung, 14, 1-62.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00403870

[22]  Horstmann, G., & Dreisbach, G. (2012). Allgemeine Psychologie 2 kompakt: Lernen, Emotion, Motivation, Gedachtnis. Weinheim: Beltz Verlag.
[23]  Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of Behavior: An Introduction to Behavior Theory. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
[24]  Hüther, G. (2009). Die Bedeutung sozialer Erfahrungen für die Strukturierung des menschlichen Gehirns. Welche sozialen Beziehungen brauchen Schüler und Lehrer? In U. Hermann (Ed.), Neurodidaktik. Grundlagen und Vorschlage für gehirngerechtes Lehren und Lernen (2th ed., pp. 41-48). Weinheim: Beltz Verlag.
[25]  Isen, A. M. (1999). Positive Affect. In T. Dalgleish, & M. J. Power (Eds.), Handbook of Cognition and Emotion (pp. 521-539). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/0470013494.ch25

[26]  James, W. (1884). What Is an Emotion? Mind, 9, 188-295.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/mind/os-IX.34.188

[27]  Jones, M. C. (1924). A B1 Laboratory Study of Fear: The Case of Peter. Pedagogical Seminary, 31, 308-315.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08856559.1924.9944851

[28]  Kamin, L. J. (1969). Predictability, Surprise, Attention, and Conditioning. In B. A. Campbell, & R. M. Church (Eds.), Punishment Aversive Behavior (pp. 279-296). New York: Appleton- Century-Crofts.
[29]  Kuhl, J., & Goschke, T. (1994). A Theory of Action Control: Mental Subsystems, Modes of Control, and Volitional Conflict-Resolution Strategies. In J. Kuhl, & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Volition and Personality: Action Versus State Orientation (pp. 93-124). Gottingen: Hogrefe.
[30]  Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective Ratings of Pictures and Instruction Manual. Technical Report A-8. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.
[31]  Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion und Adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.
[32]  LeDoux, J. E. (1998). The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life. New York: Simon & Schuster.
[33]  Levey, A. B., & Martin, I. (1975). Classical Conditioning of Human Evaluative Responses. Behavioral Research & Therapy, 13, 221-226.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(75)90026-1

[34]  Lewin, K. (1964). Die Psychologische Situation bei Lohn und Strafe. Darmstadt: Wissenscha- ftliche Buchgesellschaft.
[35]  Markowitsch, H. J. (2002). Dem Gedachtnis auf der Spur. Vom Erinnern und Vergessen. Darmstadt: Wissenschaftliche Buchgesellschaft.
[36]  Miller, G. A. (1956). The Magical Number Seven, plus or minus Two: Some Limits on our Capacity for Processing Information. Psychological Review, 63, 81-97.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0043158

[37]  Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1997). Levels of Processing versus Transfer Appropriate Processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 519-533.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(77)80016-9

[38]  Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in Personality. New York: Oxford University Press.
[39]  Ortony, A., Clore, G., & Collins, A. (1988). The Cognitive Structure of Emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511571299

[40]  Premack, D. (1963). Rate Differential Reinforcement in Monkey Manipulation. Journal for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 6, 81-89.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1963.6-81

[41]  Prinz, W. (2005). An Ideomotor Approach to Imitation. In S. Hurley, & N. Chater (Eds.), Perspectives on Imitation: From Neuroscience to Social Science (pp. 141-156). Cambridge: Bradford.
[42]  Recorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A Theory of Pavlovian Conditioning: Variations in the Effectiveness of Reinforcement and Nonreinforcement. In A. H. Black, & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory (pp. 64-99). New York: Appleton- Century-Crofts.
[43]  Roth, G. (2003). Fühlen, Denken Handeln. Frankfurt Am: Suhrkamp.
[44]  Roth, G. (2009). Warum sind Lehren und Lernen so schwierig? In U. Hermann (Ed.), Neurodidaktik. Grundlagen und Vorschlage für gehirngerechtes Lehren und Lernen (2th ed., pp. 58-68). Weinheim: Beltz Verlag.
[45]  Russell, J. A. (1991). In Defense of a Prototype Approach to Emotion Concepts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 37-47.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.60.1.37

[46]  Sachser, N. (2009). Neugier, Spiel und Lernen: Verhaltensbiologische Anmerkungen zur Kindheit. In U. Hermann (Ed.), Neurodidaktik. Grundlagen und Vorschlage für gehirngerechtes Lehren und Lernen (2th ed., pp. 19-30). Weinheim: Beltz Verlag.
[47]  Schacter, D. L., Chiao, J. Y., & Mitchell, J. P. (2003). The Seven Sins of Memory: Implications for the Self. In J. LeDoux, J. Debiece, & H. Moos (Eds.), The Self: From Soul to Brain (Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences Vol. 1001) (pp. 226-239). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.
[48]  Seligman, M. E. P. (1970). On the Generality of the Laws of Learning. Psychological Review, 77, 406-418.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0029790

[49]  Skinner, B. F. (1963). Operant Behavior. American Psychologist, 18, 503-515.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0045185

[50]  Slamecka, N. J., & Graf, P. (1978). The Generation Effect: Delineation of a Phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 592-604.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.4.6.592

[51]  Spence, K. W. (1958). A Theory of Emotionally Based Drive (D) and Its Relation to Performance in Simple Learning Situations. American Psychologist, 13, 131-141.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0045054

[52]  Sperling, G. (1960). The Information Available in Brief Visual Presentations. Psychological Monographs, 74, 1-29.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0093759

[53]  Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic Memory: From Mind to Brain. Annual Reviews of Psychology, 53, 1- 25.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114

[54]  Weiner, B. (1980). Human Motivation. New York: Holt.
[55]  Wundt, W. (1922). Vorlesungen über die Menschen und Tierseele (7th ed.). Leipzig: Voss.

Full-Text

comments powered by Disqus

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

微信:OALib Journal