[1] | Krekelberg B, Lappe M (2001) Neuronal latencies and the position of moving objects. Trends in Neurosciences 24: 335–339.
|
[2] | Nijhawan R (2002) Neural delays, visual motion and the flash-lag effect. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6: 387.
|
[3] | Nijhawan R (2008) Visual prediction: psychophysics and neurophysiology of compensation for time delays. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31: 179–198; discussion 198–239.
|
[4] | Schlag J, Schlag-Rey M (2002) Through the eye, slowly: delays and localization errors in the visual system. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3: 191–215.
|
[5] | Ogmen H, Patel SS, Bedell HE, Camuz K (2004) Differential latencies and the dynamics of the position computation process for moving targets, assessed with the flash-lag effect. Vision research 44: 2109–2128.
|
[6] | Nijhawan R (1994) Motion extrapolation in catching. Nature 370: 256–257.
|
[7] | Whitney D, Murakami I (1998) Latency difference, not spatial extrapolation. Nature Neuroscience 1: 656–657.
|
[8] | Patel SS, Ogmen H, Bedell HE, Sampath V (2000) Flash-lag effect: differential latency, not postdiction. Science 290: 1051.
|
[9] | Purushothaman G, Patel SS, Bedell HE, Ogmen H (1998) Moving ahead through differential visual latency. Nature 396: 424.
|
[10] | Baldo MV, Klein SA (1995) Extrapolation or attention shift? Nature 378: 565–566.
|
[11] | Brenner E, Smeets JB (2000) Motion extrapolation is not responsible for the flash-lag effect. Vision Research 40: 1645–1648.
|
[12] | Krekelberg B, Lappe M (2000) The position of moving objects. Science 289: 1107.
|
[13] | Eagleman DM, Sejnowski TJ (2007) Motion signals bias localization judgments: a unified explanation for the flash-lag, flash-drag, flash-jump, and Frohlich illusions. Journal of Vision 7: 3.
|
[14] | Eagleman DM, Sejnowski TJ (2000) Motion integration and postdiction in visual awareness. Science 287: 2036–2038.
|
[15] | Nijhawan R (1992) Misalignment of contours through the interaction of the apparent and real motion systems. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science 33: 1415.
|
[16] | Kerzel D (2000) Eye movements and visible persistence explain the mislocalization of the final position of a moving target. Vision Research 40: 3703–3715.
|
[17] | Koch C, Poggio T (1999) Predicting the visual world: silence is golden. Nature Neuroscience 2: 9–10.
|
[18] | Rao RP, Ballard DH (1999) Predictive coding in the visual cortex: a functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nature Neuroscience 2: 79–87.
|
[19] | Holy TE (2007) A public confession: the retina trumpets its failed predictions. Neuron 55: 831–832.
|
[20] | Schwartz G, Taylor S, Fisher C, Harris R, Berry MJ 2nd (2007) Synchronized firing among retinal ganglion cells signals motion reversal. Neuron 55: 958–969.
|
[21] | Desimone R (1998) Visual attention mediated by biased competition in extrastriate visual cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 353: 1245–1255.
|
[22] | Maus GW, Nijhawan R (2006) Forward displacements of fading objects in motion: the role of transient signals in perceiving position. Vision Research 46: 4375–4381.
|
[23] | Maus GW, Nijhawan R (2009) Going, going, gone: localizing abrupt offsets of moving objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 35: 611–626.
|
[24] | Maus GW, Nijhawan R (2008) Motion extrapolation into the blind spot. Psychological Science 19: 1087–1091.
|
[25] | Hecht E (2002) Optics. Addison-Wesley.
|
[26] | Nussbaum JJ, Pruett RC, Delori FC (1981) Historic perspectives. Macular yellow pigment. The first 200 years. Retina 1: 296–310.
|
[27] | Isobe K, Motokawa K (1955) Functional structure of the retinal fovea and Maxwell's spot. Nature 175: 306–307.
|
[28] | Komatsu H (2006) The neural mechanisms of perceptual filling-in. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 7: 220–231.
|
[29] | Magnussen S, Spillmann L, Sturzel F, Werner JS (2001) Filling-in of the foveal blue scotoma. Vision Research 41: 2961–2967.
|
[30] | Fr?hlich FW (1923) über die Messung der Empfindungszeit (Measuring the time of sensation). Zeitschrift für Sinnesphysiologie 54:
|
[31] | Kirschfeld K, Kammer T (1999) The Fr?hlich effect: a consequence of the interaction of visual focal attention and metacontrast. Vision Research 39: 3702–3709.
|
[32] | Müsseler J, Aschersleben G (1998) Localizing the first position of a moving stimulus: the Frohlich effect and an attention-shifting explanation. Perception & Psychophysics 60: 683–695.
|
[33] | Shi Z, Nijhawan R (2008) Behavioral significance of motion direction causes anisotropic flash-lag, flash-drag, flash-repulsion, and movement-mislocalization effects. Journal of Vision 8: 24 21–14.
|
[34] | Nijhawan R, Wu S (2009) Compensating time delays with neural predictions: are predictions sensory or motor? Philosophical Transactions Series A, Mathematical, Physical, and Engineering Sciences 367: 1063–1078.
|
[35] | Dassonville P (1995) Haptic localization and the internal representation of the hand in space. Experimental Brain Research 106: 434–448.
|
[36] | Nijhawan R, Kirschfeld K (2003) Analogous mechanisms compensate for neural delays in the sensory and the motor pathways: evidence from motor flash-lag. Current Biology 13: 749–753.
|
[37] | Stevens M, Yule DH, Ruxton GD (2008) Dazzle coloration and prey movement. Proceedings Biological sciences/The Royal Society 275: 2639–2643.
|
[38] | Stevens M, Merilaita S (2009) Animal camouflage: current issues and new perspectives. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological sciences 364: 423–427.
|