全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Working Memory Capacity, Inhibitory Control and the Role of L2 Proficiency in Aging L1 Dutch Speakers of Near-Native L2 English

DOI: 10.3390/brainsci3031261

Keywords: working memory capacity, inhibitory control, near-nativeness, L2 proficiency, aging

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

This paper examines the intricate relationship between working memory (WM) capacity and inhibitory control as a function of both L2 proficiency and age. In both its design and research questions, this study closely follows Gass & Lee’s work, where both L1 and L2 Reading Span Tasks (as measures of WM capacity) and L1 and L2 Stroop interference tasks (to measure inhibitory control) were administered. In this study, the test battery is augmented by both an L1 and L2 C-test of overall language proficiency. Participants were 63 L1 Dutch speakers of L2 English, who had been immersed in an L2 environment for a considerable amount of time. Their data were set off against those of 54 monolingual Dutch speakers and 56 monolingual English speakers. At the time of testing, all the bilingual participants had a near-native command of English and their L1 and L2 WM scores were not found to be significantly different. However, discrepancies did occur in Stroop test scores of inhibition, where the bilinguals performed better in their L2 English than L1 Dutch. These main effects often contradicted the results found in Gass & Lee’s study, who examined less proficient L2 learners. An aging effect was furthermore found: older subjects consistently performed more poorly on WM and inhibition tasks than their younger peers. These results can shed light on how individual factors like WM capacity and inhibitory control interact in successful late bilinguals and how these dynamics shift with advanced age.

References

[1]  Lenneberg, E.H. Biological Foundations of Language; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1967.
[2]  Singleton, D.; Lengyel, Z. The Age Factor in Second Language Acquisition; Multilingual Matters: Cleveldon, UK, 1995.
[3]  D?rnyei, Z. The Psychology of the Language Learner: Individual Differences in Second Language Acquisition; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2005.
[4]  Miyake, A.; Friedman, N. Individual Differences in Second Language Proficiency: Working Memory as Language Aptitude. In Foreign Language Learning: Psycholinguistic Studies in Training and Retention; Healy, A., Bourne, L., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: London, UK, 1998.
[5]  Cepeda, N.J.; Kramer, A.F.; Gonzalez de Sather, J.C. Changes in executive control across the lifespan: Examination of task-switching performance. Dev. Psychol. 2001, 37, 715–730, doi:10.1037/0012-1649.37.5.715.
[6]  Gass, S.; Lee, J. Working Memory Capacity, Inhibitory Control, and Proficiency in a Second Language. In Modeling Bilingualism: From Structure to Chaos; Schmid, M.S., Lowie, W., Eds.; John Benjamins: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2011.
[7]  Linck, J.A.; Schweiter, J.W.; Sunderman, G. Inhibitory control predicts language switching performance in trilingual speech production. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2012, 15, 651–662, doi:10.1017/S136672891100054X.
[8]  Glisky, E.L. Changes in Cognitive Function in Human Aging. In Brain Aging: Models, Methods, and Mechanisms; Riddle, D.R, Ed.; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2007.
[9]  Baddeley, A.D.; Hitch, G.J. Working Memory. In Recent Advances in Learning and Motivation; Bower, G.A., Ed.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA; pp. 47–90.
[10]  Baddeley, A.D. Working memory and language: An overview. J. Commun. Dis. 2003, 36, 189–208, doi:10.1016/S0021-9924(03)00019-4.
[11]  Brébion, G.; Ehrlich, M.-F.; Tardieu, H. Working memory in older subjects: Dealing with ongoing and stored information in language comprehension. Psychol. Res. 1995, 58, 225–232, doi:10.1007/BF00419637.
[12]  Just, M.A.; Carpenter, P.A. A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Pshychol. Rev. 1992, 99, 122–149, doi:10.1037/0033-295X.99.1.122.
[13]  Hegarty, M.; Shah, P.; Miyake, A. Constraints in using the dual-task methodology to specify the degree of central executive involvement in cognitive tasks. Mem. Cogn. 2007, 28, 376–385, doi:10.3758/BF03198553.
[14]  Daneman, M.; Carpenter, P.A. Individual differences in working memory and reading. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 1980, 19, 450–466, doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6.
[15]  Burke, D.M.; Shafto, M.A. Language and Aging. In The Handbook of Aging and Cognition; Craik, F.I.M., Salthouse, T.A., Eds.; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA; pp. 373–443.
[16]  Grossman, M.; Wingfield, A. Language and the aging brain: Patterns of neural compensation revealed by functional brain imaging. J. Neurophysiol. 2006, 96, 2830–2839, doi:10.1152/jn.00628.2006.
[17]  Stine, E.; Wingfield, A. Process and strategy in memory for speech among young and older adults. Psychol. Aging 1987, 2, 272–279, doi:10.1037/0882-7974.2.3.272.
[18]  Salthouse, T.A. The aging of working memory. Neuropsychology 1994, 8, 535, doi:10.1037/0894-4105.8.4.535.
[19]  Gick, M.L.; Craik, F.I.M.; Morris, R.G. Task complexity and age differences in working memory. Mem. Cogn. 1988, 16, 353–361, doi:10.3758/BF03197046.
[20]  Babcock, R.L.; Salthouse, T.A. Effects of increased processing demands on age differences in working memory. Psychol. Aging 1990, 5, 421–428, doi:10.1037/0882-7974.5.3.421.
[21]  Ehrlich, M.-F.; Brébion, G.; Tardieu, H. Working memory capacity and reading comprehension in young and older adults. Psychol. Res. 1994, 56, 110–115, doi:10.1007/BF00419718.
[22]  Service, E.; Simola, M.; Metsaenheimo, O.; Maury, S. Bilingual working memory span is affected by language skill. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2002, 14, 383–407, doi:10.1080/09541440143000140.
[23]  Van den Noort, M.; Bosch, P.; Hugdahl, K. Foreign language proficiency and working memory capacity. Eur. Psychol. 2006, 11, 289–296, doi:10.1027/1016-9040.11.4.289.
[24]  Green, D.W. Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 1998, 1, 67–81, doi:10.1017/S1366728998000133.
[25]  Paradis, M. A Neurolinguistic Theory of Bilingualism; John Benjamins: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2004.
[26]  Kroll, J.F.; Stewart, E. Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representation. J. Mem. Lang. 1994, 33, 149–174, doi:10.1006/jmla.1994.1008.
[27]  Kroll, J.F.; van Hell, J.G.; Tokowicz, N.; Green, D.W. The Revised Hierarchical Model: A critical review and assessment. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2010, 13, 373–381, doi:10.1017/S136672891000009X.
[28]  Christ, S.E.; White, D.A.; Mandernach, T.; Keys, B.A. Inhibitory control across the lifespan. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2001, 20, 653–669.
[29]  Bialystok, E.; Craik, F.I.M.; Klein, R.; Viswanathan, M. Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon Task. Psychol. Aging 2004, 19, 290–303, doi:10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290.
[30]  Hasher, L.; Zacks, R.T.; May, C.P. Inhibitory Control, Circadian Arousal, and Age. In Attention and Performance XVII; Gopher, D., Koriat, A., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1999.
[31]  Kane, M.J.; Engle, R.W. Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2003, 132, 47–70, doi:10.1037/0096-3445.132.1.47.
[32]  Oscarson, M. Item Response and Reduced Redundancy Techniques: Some Notes on Recent Developments in Language Testing. In Foreign Language Research in Cross-Cultural Perspectives; de Bot, K., Ginsberg, R.B., Kramsch, C., Eds.; John Benjamins: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1991; pp. 95–111.
[33]  Oller, J.W.; Streiff, V. Dictation: A Test of Grammar-Based Expectancies. In Testing Language Proficiency; Jones, R.L., Spolsky, B., Eds.; Center for Applied Linguistics: Arlington, VA, USA, 1975; pp. 71–82.
[34]  Gradman, H.L.; Spolsky, B. Reduced Redundancy Testing: A Progressive Report. In Testing Language Proficiency; Jones, R.L., Spolsky, B., Eds.; Center for Applied Linguistics: Arlington, VA, USA, 1975; pp. 59–66.
[35]  Raatz, U.; Klein-Braley, C. Introduction to the C-Test; Gerhard-Mercator-Universit?t: Hamburg, Germany, 1982.
[36]  Grotjahn, R. How to construct and evaluate a C-test: A discussion of some problems and some statistical analyses. Quan. Ling. 1987, 34, 219–253.
[37]  Van den Noort, M.W.M.L.; Bosch, P.; Haverkort, M.; Hugdahl, K. A standard computerized version of the Reading Span Test in different languages. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2008, 24, 35–42, doi:10.1027/1015-5759.24.1.35.
[38]  Tzelgov, J.; Henik, A.; Leiser, D. Controlling Stroop interference: Evidence from a bilingual task. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1990, 16, 760–771, doi:10.1037/0278-7393.16.5.760.
[39]  MacLeod, C.M. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychol. Bull. 1991, 109, 163–203, doi:10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163.
[40]  Keijzer, M.C.J. Last in First out? An Investigation of the Regression Hypothesis in Dutch Emigrants in Anglophone Canada; LOT Publications: Utrecht, The Netherlands, 2007.
[41]  Peters, N. The Dutch Down Under, 1606–2006; University of Western Australia Press: Crawley, Australia, 2006.
[42]  Bialystok, E.; Craik, F.I.M.; Klein, R.; Viswanathan, M. Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon Task. Psychol. Aging 2004, 19, 290–303, doi:10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290.
[43]  Hoeijmakers, M.; de Bree, E.H.; Keijzer, M.C.J. English spelling performance of Dutch grammar school students. Dutch J. Appl. Linguist. 2013. in press.

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133