[1] | Jonides J, Lewis RL, Nee D, Lustig C, Berman MG, et al. (2008) The mind and brain of short-term memory. Annual Review of Psychology 59: 193–224.
|
[2] | Oberauer K (2009) Design for a working memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory 51: 45–100.
|
[3] | Frensch PA, Rünger D (2003) Implicit learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science 12: 13–18.
|
[4] | Reber AS (1967) Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 6: 855–863.
|
[5] | Frensch PA, Miner CS (1994) Effects of presentation rate and individual differences in short-term memory. Memory and Cognition 22: 95–110.
|
[6] | Conway ARA, Getz SJ, Macnamara B, Engel de Abreu PMJ (2011) Working memory and intelligence. In: Sternberg RJ, Kaufman SB, editors. The Cambridge handbook of intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 394–418.
|
[7] | Oberauer K, Sü? HM, Wilhelm O, Wittmann WW (2003) The multiple faces of working memory: Storage, processing, supervision, and coordination. Intelligence 31: 167–193.
|
[8] | Oberauer K, Sü? HM, Wilhelm O, Wittmann WW (2008) Which working memory functions predict intelligence. Intelligence 36: 641–652.
|
[9] | Destrebecqz A, Cleeremans A (2001) Can sequence learning be implicit? New evidence with the process dissociation procedure. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 8: 343–350.
|
[10] | Destrebecqz A, Cleeremans A (2003) Temporal effects in sequence learning. In: Jiménez L, editor. Attention and implicit learning. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. pp. 181–213.
|
[11] | Cowan N (2005) Working memory capacity. NY: Psychology Press.
|
[12] | Just MA, Carpenter PA (1992) A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review 99: 122–149.
|
[13] | Kyllonen PC, Stephens DL (1990) Cognitive abilities as determinants of success in acquiring logic skill. Learning and Individual Differences 2: 129–160.
|
[14] | Rosen VM, Engle RW (1997) The role of working memory capacity in retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 126: 211–227.
|
[15] | Kane MJ, Engle RW (2003) Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 132: 47–70.
|
[16] | Daneman M, Green I (1986) Individual differences in comprehending and producing words in context. Journal of Memory and Language 25: 1–18.
|
[17] | Conway ARA, Cowan N, Bunting MF, Therriault DJ, Minkoff SRB (2002) A latent variable analysis of working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, processing speed, and general fluid intelligence. Intelligence 30: 163–184.
|
[18] | Hambrick DZ, Engle RW (2002) Effects of domain knowledge, working memory capacity, and age on cognitive performance: An investigation of the knowledge-is-power hypothesis. Cognitive Psychology 44: 339–387.
|
[19] | Oberauer K, Sü? HM, Schulze R, Wilhelm O, Wittmann WW (2000) Working memory capacity – facets of a cognitive ability construct. Personality and Individual Differences 29: 1017–1045.
|
[20] | Lewandowsky S, Oberauer K, Yang L, Ecker U (2010) A working memory test battery for MATLAB. Behavior Research Methods 42: 571–585.
|
[21] | Schvaneveldt RW, Gomez R (1998) Attention and probabilistic sequence learning. Psychological Research 61: 175–190.
|
[22] | Cohen A, Ivry RI, Keele SW (1990) Attention and structure in sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 16: 17–30.
|
[23] | Fu Q, Fu X, Dienes Z (2008) Implicit sequence learning and conscious awareness. Consciousness and Cognition 17: 185–202.
|
[24] | Wilkinson L, Shanks DR (2004) Intentional control and implicit sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 30: 354–369.
|
[25] | Burgess N, Hitch G (2005) Computational models of working memory: Putting long-term memory into context. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9: 535–541.
|
[26] | Unsworth N, Engle RW (2007) The nature of individual differences in working memory capacity: Active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from secondary memory. Psychological Review 114: 104–132.
|
[27] | Hebb DO (1961) Distinctive features of learning in the higher animal. In: Delafresnaye JE, editor. Brain mechanisms and learning. NY: Oxford University Press.
|
[28] | Oberauer K (2010) Declarative and procedural working memory: Common principles, common capacity limits? Psychologica Belgica 50: 277–308.
|
[29] | Hitch GJ, Flude B, Burgess N (2009) Slave to the rhythm: Experimental tests of a model for verbal short-term memory and long-term sequence learning. Journal of Memory and Language 61: 97–111.
|
[30] | Frensch PA, Wenke D, Rünger D (1999) A secondary tone-counting task suppresses expression of knowledge in the serial reaction task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 25: 260–274.
|
[31] | Shanks D, Rowland L, Ranger M (2005) Attentional load and implicit sequence learning. Psychological Research 69: 369–382.
|
[32] | Nissen MJ, Bullemer P (1987) Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology 19: 1–32.
|
[33] | Stadler M (1995) Role of attention in implicit learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21: 674–685.
|
[34] | Cleeremans A, Jiménez L (2002) Implicit learning and consciousness: A graded, dynamic perspective. In: French RM, Cleeremans A, editors. Implicit learning and consciousness: An empirical, computational and philosophical consensus in the making. Hove: Psychology Press. pp. 1–40.
|
[35] | Cleeremans A (2008) Consciousness: The radical plasticity thesis. Progress in Brain Research 168: 19–33.
|
[36] | Sutton RS, Barto AG (1981) Toward a modern theory of adaptive networks: Expectation and prediction. Psychological Review 88: 135–171.
|
[37] | Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR (1997) A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275: 1593–1599.
|
[38] | Feldman J, Kerr B, Streissguth AP (1995) Correlational analyses of procedural and declarative learning performance. Intelligence 20: 87–114.
|
[39] | Unsworth N, Engle RW (2005) Individual differences in working memory capacity and learning: Evidence from the serial reaction time task. Memory and Cognition 33: 213–220.
|
[40] | Kaufman SB, DeYoung CG, Gray JR, Jiménez L, Brown J, et al. (2010) Implicit learning as an ability. Cognition 116: 321–340.
|
[41] | Keele SW, Ivry RB, Mayr U, Hazeltine E, Heuer H (2003) The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation. Psychological Review 110: 316–339.
|
[42] | Brainard DH (1997) The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision 10: 433–436.
|
[43] | Pelli DG (1997) The Video Toolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision 10: 437–442.
|
[44] | Ecker UKH, Lewandowsky S, Oberauer K, Chee AEH (2010) The components of working memory updating: An experimental decomposition and individual differences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 36: 170–189.
|
[45] | Reed J, Johnson P (1994) Assessing implicit learning with indirect tests: Determining what is learnt about sequence structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 20: 585–594.
|
[46] | Baddeley AD, Conrad R, Hull AJ (1965) Predictability and immediate memory for consonant sequences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 17: 175–177.
|
[47] | Majerus S, Van der Linden M, Mulder L, Meulemans T, Peters F (2004) Verbal short-term memory reflects the sublexical organization of the phonological language network: Evidence from an incidental phonotactic learning paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language 51: 297–306.
|
[48] | Botvinick M, Bylsma L (2005) Regularization in short-term memory for serial order. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2: 351–358.
|
[49] | Kane MJ, Bleckley MK, Conway ARA, Engle RW (2001) A controlled-attention view of WM capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 130: 169–183.
|
[50] | Conway ARA, Cowan N, Bunting MF (2001) The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: The importance of WM capacity. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 8: 331–335.
|
[51] | Szmalec A, Verbruggen F, Vandierendonck A, Kemps E (2011) Control of interference during working memory updating. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 37: 137–151.
|
[52] | Frensch PA, Lin J, Buchner A (1998) Learning vs. behavioral expression of the learned: The effects of a secondary tone-counting task on implicit learning in the serial reaction task. Psychological Research 61: 83–98.
|
[53] | Abrahamse EL, Jiménez L, Verwey WB, Clegg BA (2010) Representing serial action and perception. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 17: 603–623.
|
[54] | Gaschler R, Frensch PA, Cohen A, Wenke D (2012) Implicit sequence learning based on instructed action codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 38: 1389–1407.
|
[55] | Schuck NW, Gaschler R, Keisler A, Frensch PA (2012) Position-item associations play a role in the acquisition of order knowledge in an implicit serial reaction time task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 38: 440–456.
|
[56] | Wharton CM, Holyoak KJ, Downing PE, Lange TE, Wickens TD, et al. (1994) Below the surface: Analogical similarity and retrieval competition in reminding. Cognitive Psychology 26: 64–101.
|
[57] | Conway ARA, Engle RW (1994) Working memory and retrieval: A resource-dependent inhibition model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 123: 354–373.
|
[58] | Majerus S, Poncelet M, Van der Linden M, Weekes B (2008) Lexical learning in bilingual adults: The relative importance of short-term memory for serial order and phonological knowledge. Cognition 107: 395–419.
|
[59] | Botvinick M (2005) Effects of domain-specific knowledge on memory for serial order. Cognition 97: 135–151.
|
[60] | Herwig A, Waszak F (2012) Action-effect bindings and ideomotor learning in intention- and stimulus-based actions. Frontiers in Psychology 3: 444 doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00444.
|
[61] | Colzato LS, Raffone A, Hommel B (2006) What do we learn from binding features? Evidence for multilevel feature integration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 32: 705–716.
|
[62] | Monsell S (2003) Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7: 134–140.
|
[63] | Szmalec A, Vandierendonck A, Kemps E (2005) Response selection involves executive control: Evidence from the selective interference paradigm. Memory and Cognition 33: 531–541.
|