[1] | Neill WT (1977) Inhibitory and facilitatory processes in selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 3: 444–450.
|
[2] | Tipper SP (1985) The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology 37: 571–590.
|
[3] | Wong KFE (2000) Dissociative prime-probe contextual similarity effects on negative priming and repetition priming: A challenge to episodic retrieval as a unified account of negative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 26: 1411–1422.
|
[4] | Fox E (1995) Negative priming from ignored distractors in visual selection: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2: 145–173.
|
[5] | May CP, Kane MJ, Hasher L (1995) Determinants of negative priming. Psychological Bulletin 118: 35–54.
|
[6] | Chun MM (1997) Types and tokens in visual processing: A double dissociation between the attentional blink and repetition blindness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 23: 738–755.
|
[7] | Chun MM, Potter MC (1995) A two-stage model for multiple target detection in rapid serial visual presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 21: 109–127.
|
[8] | Raymond JE, Shapiro KL, Arnell KM (1992) Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: An attentional blink? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 18: 849–860.
|
[9] | Kanwisher N (1987) Repletion blindness: Type recognition without token individuation. Cognition 27: 117–143.
|
[10] | Wong KFE (2002) The relationship between attentional blink and psychological refractory period. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance 28: 54–71.
|
[11] | Neill WT (1979) Switching attention within and between categories: Evidence for intracategory inhibition. Memory & Cognition 7: 283–290.
|
[12] | Neill WT (1997) Episodic retrieval in negative priming and repetition priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 23: 1291–1305.
|
[13] | Neill WT, Valdes LA (1992) The persistence of negative priming: Steady-state or decay? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 18: 565–576.
|
[14] | Neill WT, Valdes LA (1996) Facilitatory and inhibitory aspects of attention. In: Kramer AF, Coles MGH, Logan GD, editors. Converging operations in the study of visual selective attention. pp. 77–106.
|
[15] | Lowe DG (1979) Strategies, context, and the mechanism of response inhibition. Memory & Cognition 7: 382–389.
|
[16] | Park J, Kanwisher N (1994) Negative priming for spatial location: Identity mismatching, not distractor inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 20: 613–623.
|
[17] | Milliken B, Joordens S, Merikle PM, Seiffert AE (1998) Selective attention: A reevaluation of the implications of negative priming. Psychological Review 105: 203–229.
|
[18] | Loach D, Mari-Beffa P (2003) Post-target inhibition: A temporal binding mechanism? Visual Cognition 10: 513–526.
|
[19] | Harris IM, Benito CT, Dux PE (2010) Priming from distractors in rapid serial visual presentation is modulated by image properties and attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 36: 1595–1608.
|
[20] | Kihara K, Yagi Y, Takeda Y, Kawahara JI (2011) Distractor devaluation effect in the attentional blink: Direct evidence for distractor inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 37: 168–179.
|
[21] | Maki WS, Couture T, Frigen K, Lien D (1997) Sources of the attentional blink during rapid serial visual presentation: Perceptual interference and retrieval competition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 23: 1393–1411.
|
[22] | Birnbaum MH (1974) The nonadditivity of personality impressions. Journal of Experimental Psychology 102: 543–561.
|
[23] | Mellers BA, Cooke ADJ (1994) Trade-offs depend on attribute range. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 20: 1055–1067.
|
[24] | Kanwisher N (1987) Repletion blindness: Type recognition without token individuation. Cognition 27: 117–143.
|
[25] | Kanwisher N (1991) Repetition blindness and illusory conjunctions: Errors in binding visual types with visual tokens. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 17: 404–421.
|
[26] | Kanwisher N, Potter MC (1990) Repetition blindness: Levels of processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 16: 30–47.
|
[27] | Armstrong IT, Mewhort DJK (1995) Repetition deficit in rapid-serial-visual-presentation displays: Encoding failure or retrieval failure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 21: 1044–1052.
|
[28] | Fagot C, Pashler H (1995) Repetition blindness: Perception or memory failure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 21: 275–292.
|
[29] | Whittlesea BWA, Masson MEJ (2005) Repetition blindness in rapid lists: Activation and inhibition versus construction and attribution. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 31: 54–67.
|
[30] | Whittlesea BWA, Podrouzek KW (1995) Repeated events in rapid lists: Part 2. Remembering repetition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21: 1688–1697.
|
[31] | Morris AL, Harris CL (2004) Repetition blindness: Out of sight or out of mind? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 30: 913–922.
|
[32] | Wong KFE, Chen H-C (2009) Forward and backward repetition blindness in speed and accuracy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 35: 913–922.
|
[33] | Haberlandt KF, Graesser AC (1985) Component processes in text comprehension and some of their introductions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 114: 357–374.
|
[34] | Haberlandt KF, Graesser AC (1989) Processing of new arguments at clause boundaries. Memory and Cognition 17: 186–193.
|
[35] | Just MA, Carpenter PA, Woolley JD (1982) Paradigms and processes in reading comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 111: 228–238.
|
[36] | Wong KFE, Chen H-C (1999) Orthographic and phonological processing in reading Chinese. Language and Cognitive Processes 14: 461–480.
|
[37] | Frederick SW, Mochon D (2012) A scale distortion theory of anchoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 141: 124–133.
|
[38] | Wong KFE, Kwong JYY (2005) Comparing two tiny giants or two huge dwarfs? Preference reversals owing to number size framing. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 98: 54–65.
|
[39] | Wong KFE, Kwong JYY (2005) Between-individual comparisons in performance evaluation: A perspective from prospect theory. Journal of Applied Psychology 90: 284–294.
|
[40] | Wang XM, Wong KFE, Kwong JYY (2010) The roles of rater goals and ratee performance levels in the distortion of performance rating. Journal of Applied Psychology 95: 546–561.
|
[41] | Wong KFE, Kwong JYY (2007) Effects of rater goals on rating patterns in performance evaluation: Evidence from an experimental field study. Journal of Applied Psychology 92: 577–585.
|
[42] | Tipper SP, Cranston M (1985) Selective attention and priming: Inhibitory and facilitatory effects of ignored primes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology 37: 591–611.
|
[43] | Dux PE, Coltheart V, Harris IM (2006) On the fate of distractor stimuli in rapid serial visual presentation. Cognition 99: 355–382.
|
[44] | Dux PE, Harris IM (2007) On the failure of distractor inhibition in the attentional blink. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 14: 723–728.
|
[45] | Lavie N, Fox E (2000) The role of perceptual load in negative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 26: 1038–1052.
|
[46] | Lavie N, Hirst A, De Fockert JW, Viding E (2004) Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 133: 339–354.
|
[47] | Folk CL, Leber AB, Egeth HE (2008) Top-down control settings and the attentional blink: Evidence for nonspatial contingent capture. Visual Cognition 16: 616–642.
|
[48] | Frings C, Wühr P (2007) Prime-display offset modulates negative priming only for easy-selection tasks. Memory & Cognition 35: 504–513.
|
[49] | Houghton G, Tipper SP, Weaver B, Shore DI (1996) Inhibition and interference in selective attention: Some tests of a neural network model. Visual Cognition 3: 119–164.
|
[50] | Jolicoeur P (1998) Modulation of the attentional blink by online response selection: Evidence from speeded and unspeeded Task-sub-1 decisions. Memory & Cognition 26: 1014–1032.
|
[51] | Raymond JE, Shapiro KL, Arnell KM (1995) Similarity determines the attentional blink. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 21: 653–662.
|
[52] | Shapiro KL, Raymond JE, Arnell KM (1994) Attention to visual pattern information produces the attentional blink in rapid serial visual presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 20: 357–371.
|
[53] | Potter MC, Chun MM, Banks BS, Muckenhoupt M (1998) Two attentional deficits in serial target search: The visual attentional blink and an amodal task-switch deficit. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 24: 979–992.
|
[54] | Visser TAW, Bischof WF, Di Lollo V (1999) Attentional switching in spatial and nonspatial domains: Evidence from the attentional blink. Psychological Bulletin 125: 458–469.
|
[55] | Jolicoeur P, Dell'Acqua R (1998) The demonstration of short-term consolidation. Cognitive Psychology 36: 138–202.
|
[56] | Pashler H (1989) Dissociations and dependencies between speed and accuracy: Evidence for a two-component theory of divided attention in simple tasks. Cognitive Psychology 21: 469–514.
|
[57] | Fox E, De Fockert JW (1998) Negative priming depends on prime-probe similarity: Evidence for episodic retrieval. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 5: 107–113.
|
[58] | Hunt RR, Elliott JM (1980) The role of nonsemantic information in memory: Orthographic distinctiveness effects on retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 109: 49–74.
|
[59] | Hunt RR, Mitchell DB (1982) Independent effects of semantic and nonsemantic distinctiveness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition 8: 81–87.
|
[60] | Rajaram S (1996) Perceptual effects on remembering: Recollective processes in picture recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition 22: 365–377.
|
[61] | Rajaram S (1998) The effects of conceptual salience and perceptual distinctiveness on conscious recollection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 5: 71–78.
|
[62] | Logan GD (1988) Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review 95: 492–527.
|
[63] | Logan GD (1990) Repetition priming and automaticity: Common underlying mechanisms? Cognitive Psychology 22: 1–35.
|
[64] | Kane MJ, May CP, Hasher L, Rahhal T (1997) Dual mechanisms of negative priming. Journal Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 23: 632–650.
|
[65] | Tipper SP (2001) Does negative priming reflect inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integration of conflicting view. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 54: 321–343.
|
[66] | Tse C-S, Hutchison KA, Li Y (2011) Effects of contextual similarity and target-repetition proportion on negative priming in RT distributional analyses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 37: 180–192.
|
[67] | Frings C, Spence C (2011) Increased perceptual and conceptual processing difficulty makes the immeasurable measurable: Negative priming in the absence of probe distractors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 37: 72–84.
|
[68] | Moore CM (1994) Negative priming depends on probe-trial conflict: Where has all the inhibition gone? Perception & Psychophysics 56: 133–147.
|