|
数学性别刻板印象威胁干预经验及其教育启示
|
Abstract:
性别刻板印象威胁指个体因担忧自身表现可能验证所属群体的负面标签而产生的心理压力,其在数学教育领域尤为显著:女性在感知数学能力的性别差异时,往往因威胁效应导致测试成绩下降、学科兴趣衰减及职业选择回避。本研究基于“个体–群体–环境”多层次框架,从“微观–中观–宏观”层面系统整合数学性别刻板印象威胁的干预路径,并提出若干教育启示。
Gender stereotype threat refers to the psychological stress experienced by individuals who fear that their performance may confirm negative stereotypes associated with their social group. This phenomenon manifests particularly prominently in mathematics education, where women’s awareness of gender differences in perceived mathematical competence often leads to measurable consequences: diminished test performance, reduced academic engagement, and avoidance of STEM-related career pathways due to the threat’s psychological effects. Grounded in a tripartite “individual-group-environment” analytical framework, this study systematically synthesizes intervention strategies for mitigating mathematics-related gender stereotype threats across micro-, meso-, and macro-level dimensions, while proposing actionable recommendations for educational practice.
[1] | 俞国良. 社会心理学前沿[M]. 北京: 北京师范大学出版社, 2010. |
[2] | Steele, C.M. and Aronson, J. (1995) Stereotype Threat and the Intellectual Test Performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797-811. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797 |
[3] | 张宝山, 袁菲, 徐靓鸽. 刻板印象威胁效应的消除: 干预策略及其展望[J]. 心理科学, 2014(1): 197-204. |
[4] | Spencer, S.J., Steele, C.M. and Quinn, D.M. (1999) Stereotype Threat and Women’s Math Performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 4-28. https://doi.org/10.1006/jesp.1998.1373 |
[5] | 阮小林, 张庆林, 杜秀敏, 等. 刻板印象威胁效应研究回顾与展望[J]. 心理科学进展, 2009, 17(4): 836-844. |
[6] | Sheltzer, J.M. and Smith, J.C. (2014) Elite Male Faculty in the Life Sciences Employ Fewer Women. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111, 10107-10112. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1403334111 |
[7] | Speer, J.D. (2023) Bye Bye Ms. American Sci: Women and the Leaky STEM Pipeline. Economics of Education Review, 93, Article 102371. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.econedurev.2023.102371 |
[8] | Koch, S.C., Müller, S.M. and Sieverding, M. (2008) Women and Computers. Effects of Stereotype Threat on Attribution of Failure. Computers & Education, 51, 1795-1803. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2008.05.007 |
[9] | Keller, J. (2002) Blatant Stereotype Threat and Women’s Math Performance: Self-Handicapping as a Strategic Means to Cope with Obtrusive Negative Performance Expectations. Sex Roles, 47, 193-198. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1021003307511 |
[10] | Johns, M., Schmader, T. and Martens, A. (2005) Knowing Is Half the Battle: Teaching Stereotype Threat as a Means of Improving Women’s Math Performance. Psychological Science, 16, 175-179. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00799.x |
[11] | Logel, C., Iserman, E.C., Davies, P.G., Quinn, D.M. and Spencer, S.J. (2009) The Perils of Double Consciousness: The Role of Thought Suppression in Stereotype Threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 299-312. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2008.07.016 |
[12] | Bandura, A. (1986) Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Socialcognitive Theory. Englewood Cliffs, Prentice Hall. |
[13] | Yeager, D.S., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Garcia, J., Apfel, N., Brzustoski, P., Master, A., et al. (2014) Breaking the Cycle of Mistrust: Wise Interventions to Provide Critical Feedback across the Racial Divide. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 804-824. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033906 |
[14] | Gardner, H. (1983) Frames of Mind: The Theory of Multiple Intelligences. Basic Books. |
[15] | Doise, W. (1978) Groups and Individuals: Explanations in Social Psychology. Cambridge University Press. |
[16] | Rosenthal, H.E.S. and Crisp, R.J. (2006) Reducing Stereotype Threat by Blurring Intergroup Boundaries. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 501-511. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167205281009 |
[17] | Rosenthal, H.E.S., Crisp, R.J. and Suen, M. (2007) Improving Performance Expectancies in Stereotypic Domains: Task Relevance and the Reduction of Stereotype Threat. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 586-597. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.379 |
[18] | Wood, J.V. (1989) Theory and Research Concerning Social Comparisons of Personal Attributes. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 231-248. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.106.2.231 |
[19] | Gresky, D.M., Eyck, L.L.T., Lord, C.G. and McIntyre, R.B. (2005) Effects of Salient Multiple Identities on Women’s Performance under Mathematics Stereotype Threat. Sex Roles, 53, 703-716. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-005-7735-2 |
[20] | Marx, D.M. and Roman, J.S. (2002) Female Role Models: Protecting Women’s Math Test Performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1183-1193. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672022812004 |
[21] | Shih, M., Pittinsky, T.L. and Ambady, N. (1999) Stereotype Susceptibility: Identity Salience and Shifts in Quantitative Performance. Psychological Science, 10, 80-83. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00111 |
[22] | Liu, S., Liu, P., Wang, M. and Zhang, B. (2021) Effectiveness of Stereotype Threat Interventions: A Meta-Analytic Review. Journal of Applied Psychology, 106, 921-949. https://doi.org/10.1037/apl0000770 |
[23] | Aish, N., Asare, P. and Miskioglu, E.E. (2018). People Like Me: Providing Relatable and Realistic Role Models for Underrepresented Minorities in STEM to Increase Their Motivation and Likelihood of Success. 2018 IEEE Integrated STEM Education Conference (ISEC), Princeton, 10 March 2018. https://doi.org/10.1109/isecon.2018.8340510 |
[24] | Cherney, I.D. and Campbell, K.L. (2011) A League of Their Own: Do Single-Sex Schools Increase Girls’ Participation in the Physical Sciences? Sex Roles, 65, 712-724. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-011-0013-6 |
[25] | Schmader, T. (2023) Gender Inclusion and Fit in STEM. Annual Review of Psychology, 74, 219-243. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-032720-043052 |
[26] | Cheryan, S., Plaut, V.C., Davies, P.G. and Steele, C.M. (2009) Ambient Belonging: How Stereotypical Cues Impact Gender Participation in Computer Science. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 1045-1060. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016239 |
[27] | Eccles, J.S. (1994) Understanding Women’s Educational and Occupational Choices: Applying the Eccles Et Al. Model of Achievement-Related Choices. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 585-609. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1994.tb01049.x |
[28] | Wennerås, C. and Wold, A. (1997) Nepotism and Sexism in Peer-Review. Nature, 387, 341-343. https://doi.org/10.1038/387341a0 |
[29] | Burkam, D.T., Lee, V.E. and Smerdon, B.A. (1997) Gender and Science Learning Early in High School: Subject Matter and Laboratory Experiences. American Educational Research Journal, 34, 297-331. https://doi.org/10.3102/00028312034002297 |
[30] | Entwisle, D.R., Alexander, K.L. and Olson, L.S. (1994) The Gender Gap in Math: Its Possible Origins in Neighborhood Effects. American Sociological Review, 59, 822-838. https://doi.org/10.2307/2096370 |
[31] | Becker, J.R. (1981) Differential Treatment of Females and Males in Mathematics Classes. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 12, 40-53. https://doi.org/10.2307/748657 |
[32] | Kelly, A. (1988) Gender Differences in Teacher-Pupil Interactions: A Meta-Analytic Review. Research in Education, 39, 1-23. https://doi.org/10.1177/003452378803900101 |
[33] | 李红武, 孟小红. 数学-性别刻板印象威胁的教育思考[J]. 南阳师范学院学报, 2019, 18(4): 51-54. |
[34] | 宋淑娟, 刘华山. 反刻板印象信息对减弱数学-性别刻板印象威胁效应的作用[J]. 中国临床心理学杂志, 2014, 22(3): 386-389. |
[35] | 刘蕴坤, 陶沙. 数学成就的性别差异[J]. 心理科学进展, 2012(12): 1980-1990. |
[36] | Dweck, C.S. (2006) Mindset: The New Psychology of Success. Ballantine Books. |