全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

错误记忆的跨文化研究中实验法的运用
The Application of Experimental Methods in Cross-Cultural Studies of False Memories

DOI: 10.12677/ASS.2024.132174, PP. 1284-1289

Keywords: 错误记忆,跨文化研究,认知心理学,实验法
False Memory
, Cross-Culture Study, Cognitive Psychology, Experimental Method

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

文化会影响个体的认知,不同文化背景下的人群往往会因为社会、地理等因素拥有不同的认知习惯。错误记忆是指人们会回忆或再认那些没有出现过的事件,或者对所经历过事件的错误回忆。在全球化趋势越发明显的情况下,探究文化对错误记忆的影响既可以有效减少在跨文化交际中的误解,也能对错误记忆的产生机制有进一步理解。作为认知心理学基础的实验法对人类的认识过程进行模型建构有着极为重要的作用。文章列举了通过实验法求得的关于错误记忆中跨文化研究所取得的成果以及未来能够进一步研究的方向。
Culture can affect individual cognition, and people from different cultural backgrounds often have different cognitive habits due to social and geographical factors. False memory refers to people’s recall or recognition of events that have not occurred, or the erroneous recall of events that have been experienced. In the context of increasing globalization, exploring the influence of culture on false memory can not only effectively reduce misunderstandings in cross-cultural communication, but also further understand the mechanism of false memory. As the foundation of cognitive psychology, experimental methods play an extremely important role in modeling human cognitive processes. This article lists the results obtained through experimental methods in cross-cultural research on false memory, as well as directions for further research in the future.

References

[1]  Schacter, D.L., Norman, K.A. and Koutstaal, W. (1998) The Cognitive Neuroscience of Constructive Memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 289-318.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.289
[2]  Jacoby, L.L., Wahlheim, C.N., Rhodes, M.G., Daniels, K.A. and Rogers, C.S. (2010) Learning to Diminish the Effects of Proactive Interference: Reducing False Memory for Young and Older Adults. Memory & Cognition, 38, 820-829.
https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.6.820
[3]  Kloft, L., Otgaar, H., Blokland, A., Monds, L.A., Toennes, S.W., Loftus, E.F. and Ramaekers, J.G. (2020) Cannabis Increases Susceptibility to False Memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 117, 4585-4589.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1920162117
[4]  Peters, M.J., Hauschildt, M., Moritz, S. and Jelinek, L. (2013) Impact of Emotionality on Memory and Meta-Memory in Schizophrenia Using Video Sequences. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 44, 77-83.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2012.07.003
[5]  De keersmaecker, J. and Roets, A. (2017) “Fake News”: Incorrect, but Hard to Correct. The Role of Cognitive Ability on the Impact of False Information on Social Impressions. Intelligence, 65, 107-110.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2017.10.005
[6]  Neisser, U. (1967) Cognitive Psychology. Appleton Century Crofts. New York.
[7]  陈立. 对心理学中实验法的估价问题[J]. 心理科学通讯, 1966(1): 34-36.
[8]  Chua, H.F., Boland, J.E. and Nisbett, R.E. (2005) Cultural Variation in Eye Movements during Scene Perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 12629-12633.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0506162102
[9]  Nisbett, T.M. and R.E. (2001) Attending Holistically Versus Analytically Comparing the Context Sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 922-934.
https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.81.5.922
[10]  Suh, E., Diener, E., Oishi, S. and Triandis, H.C. (1998) The Shifting Basis of Life Satisfaction Judgments Across Cultures: Emotions Versus Norms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 482-493.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.482
[11]  Uchida, Y., Norasakkunkit, V. and Kitayama, S. (2004). Cultural Constructions of Happiness: Theory and Empirical Evidence. Journal of Happiness Studies, 5, 223-239.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-004-8785-9
[12]  Koo, M. and Oishi, S. (2009) False Memory and the Associative Network of Happiness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 212-220.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167208327191
[13]  Boduroglu, A., Shah, P. and Nisbett, R.E. (2009) Cultural Differences in Allocation of Attention in Visual Information Processing. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40, 349-360.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022108331005
[14]  Schwartz, A.J., Boduroglu, A. and Gutchess, A.H. (2014) Cross-Cultural Differences in Categorical Memory Errors. Cognitive Science, 38, 997-1007.
https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12109
[15]  Wang, J., Otgaar, H., Santtila, P., Shen, X. and Zhou, C. (2021) How Culture Shapes Constructive False Memory. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 10, 24-32.
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0101792
[16]  Uleman, J.S., Adil Saribay, S. and Gonzalez, C.M. (2008) Spontaneous Infe-rences, Implicit Impressions, and Implicit Theories. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 329-360.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093707
[17]  Shimizu, Y., Lee, H. and Uleman, J.S. (2017) Culture as Automatic Processes for Making Meaning: Spontaneous Trait Inferences. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 69, 79-85.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2016.08.003
[18]  Na, J. and Kitayama, S. (2011) Spontaneous Trait Inference Is Culture-Specific: Behavioral and Neural Evidence. Psychological Science, 22, 1025-1032.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611414727

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133