|
进化相关威胁刺激的注意偏向及记忆优势
|
Abstract:
[1] | 柴江霞, 肖玉珠, 姚伟莉, 赵笑梅(2015). 威胁信息的记忆优势. 心理科学, 38(2), 315-320. |
[2] | 牟柳, 刘尚礼(2017). 威胁刺激类型对不同特质焦虑水平跆拳道运动员视觉搜索效率的影响. 西南师范大学学报(自然科学版), 42(8), 102-106. |
[3] | 彭聃龄(2012). 普通心理学(第4版). 北京: 北京师范大学出版社. |
[4] | 王福兴, 李文静, 颜志强, 段朝辉, 李卉(2015). 幼儿对威胁性刺激蛇的注意觉察: 来自眼动证据. 心理学报, 47(6), 774-786. |
[5] | 王乾东, 李庆功, 陈凯凯, 傅根跃(2015). 不可信面孔在视知觉加工中的注意偏向: 来自眼动的证据. 心理科学进展, 23(9), 1508-1513. |
[6] | 文少司(2015). 恐惧模块假说: 来自个体对枪与蛇的注意加工的证据. 硕士学位论文, 长沙: 湖南师范大学. |
[7] | 毋嫘, 林冰心, 蒋娜, 赵亚萍(2016). 高焦虑个体对威胁性刺激的注意偏向特点. 心理与行为研究, 14(6), 760-764. |
[8] | 袁小钧, 崔晓霞, 曹正操, 阚红, 王晓, 汪亚珉(2018). 虚拟仿真场景中威胁性视觉刺激搜索的注意偏向效应. 心理学报, 50(6), 622-636. |
[9] | Amir, N., Elias, J., Klumpp, H., & Przeworski, A. (2003). Attentional Bias to Threat in Social Phobia: Facilitated Processing of Threat or Difficulty Disengaging Attention from Threat? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 1325-1335.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00039-1 |
[10] | Azarian, B., Esser, E. G., & Peterson, M. S. (2016). Evidence from the Eyes: Threatening Postures Hold Attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 764-770. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0942-0 |
[11] | Boyer, P., & Bergstrom, B. (2011). Threat-Detection in Child Development: An Evolutionary Perspective. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1034-1041. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.08.010 |
[12] | Brown, C., El-Deredy, W., & Blanchette, I. (2010). Attentional Modulation of Visual-Evoked Potentials by Threat: Investigating the Effect of Evolutionary Relevance. Brain and Cognition, 74, 281-287.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2010.08.008 |
[13] | Fox, E., Griggs, L., & Mouchlianitis, E. (2007). The Detection of Fear-Relevant Stimuli: Are Guns Noticed as Quickly as Snakes? Emotion, 7, 691-696. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.691 |
[14] | Gernot, H. (2009). Visual Search for Schematic Affective Faces: Stability and Variability of Search Slopes with Different Instances. Cognition & Emotion, 23, 355-379. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930801976523 |
[15] | Goodwin, H., Eagleson, C., Mathews, A., Yiend, J., & Hirsch, C. R. (2017). Automaticity of Attentional Bias to Threat in High and Low Worriers. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 41, 479-488. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-016-9818-5 |
[16] | Hakamata, Y., Lissek, S., Bar-Haim, Y., Britton, J. C., Fox, N. A., Leibenluft, E., Ernst, M., & Pine, D. S. (2010). Attention Bias Modification Treatment: A Meta-Analysis toward the Establishment of Novel Treatment for Anxiety. Biological Psychiatry, 68, 982-990. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.021 |
[17] | Koster, E. H. W., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., Vanvolsem, P., & De Houwer, J. (2007). A Time-Course Analysis of Attentional Cueing by Threatening Scenes. Experimental Psychology, 54, 161-171.
https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.54.2.161 |
[18] | Lipp, O. V., & Waters, A. M. (2007). When Danger Lurks in the Background: Attentional Capture by Animal Fear-Relevant Distractors Is Specific and Selectively Enhanced by Animal Fear. Emotion, 7, 192-200.
https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.192 |
[19] | LoBue, V., & Matthews, K. (2014). The Snake in the Grass Revisited: An Experimental Comparison of Threat Detection Paradigms. Cognition & Emotion, 28, 22-35. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2013.790783 |
[20] | Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2006). Time Course of Attentional Bias for Fear-Relevant Pictures in Spider-Fearful Individuals. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 44, 1241-1250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2006.05.003 |
[21] | Nairne, J. S., Pandeirada, J. N. S., & Thompson, S. R.(2008). Adaptive Memory: The Comparative Value of Survival Processing. Psychological Science, 19, 176-180. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02064.x |
[22] | Ohman, A. (2009). Of Snakes and Faces: An Evolutionary Perspective on the Psychology of Fear. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 50, 543-552. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00784.x |
[23] | Ohman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion Drives Attention: Detecting the Snake in the Grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 466-478. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466 |
[24] | Peira, N., Golkar, A., ?hman, A., Anders, S., & Wiens, S. (2012). Emotional Responses in Spider Fear Are Closely Related to Picture Awareness. Cognition and Emotion, 26, 252-260. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2011.579087 |
[25] | Wells, T. T., Beevers, C. G., Robison, A. E., & Ellis, A. J. (2010). Gaze Behavior Predicts Memory Bias for Angry Facial Expressions in Stable Dysphoria. Emotion, 10, 894-902. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020022 |
[26] | Yamagishi, T., Tanida, S., Mashima, R., Shimoma, E., & Kanazawa, S. (2003). You Can Judge a Book by Its Cover: Evidence That Cheaters May Look Different from Cooperators. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 290-301.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(03)00035-7 |
[27] | Zsido, A. N., Bernath, L., Labadi, B., & Deak, A. (2018). Count on Arousal: Introducing a New Method for Investigating the Effects of Emotional Valence and arousal on Visual Search Performance. Psychological Research, 84, 1-14.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-0974-y |
[28] | Zsido, A. N., Deak, A., Losonci, A., Stecina, D., Arato, A., & Bernath, L. (2018). Investigating Evolutionary Constraints on the Detection of Threatening Stimuli in Preschool Children. Acta Psychologica, 185, 166-171. |