Social exclusion has a broad effect on individuals, including basic cognitive function, emotion and behaviour. Studies using the Cyberball game showed that social exclusion influenced executive control. However, the effects of different paradigms on subjects are different. The present study aimed to explore the effect of social exclusion through the life-alone paradigm. Participants were required to complete a questionnaire, and then they randomly received fake feedback (lonely or happy) about their future social lives. To test the cognitive control of the two groups, event related potential (ERP) was recorded when participants completed a letter flanker task. The results showed that compared with included subjects, excluded subjects had a larger congruency effect and showed a smaller N2 component in both congruent and incongruent trials; additionally, they did not show a larger P3 effect in incongruent compared to congruent trials. These results indicated that social exclusion decreased subjects’ cognitive control ability, including conflict detection and response inhibition. Time-frequency results found that ERSP magnitudes of the alpha band were significantly smaller in incongruent than congruent trials for included participants, but this was not the case for excluded subjects. This finding suggested that excluded subjects might have no available resources to resolve conflict in incongruent trials. Further, compared to the ostracism paradigm, future rejection led to a decline in conflict detection ability.
References
[1]
Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Ciarocco, N. J., & Twenge, J. M. (2005). Social Exclusion Impairs Self-Regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 589-604. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.88.4.589
[2]
Baumeister, R. F., Twenge, J. M., & Nuss, C. K. (2002). Effects of Social Exclusion on Cognitive Processes: Anticipated Aloneness Reduces Intelligent Thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 817-827. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.4.817
[3]
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict Monitoring and Cognitive Control. Psychological Review, 108, 624-652. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624
[4]
Campbell, W. K., Krusemark, E. A., Dyckman, K. A., Brunell, A. B., McDowell, J. E., Twenge, J. M., & Clementz, B. A. (2006). A Magnetoencephalography Investigation of Neural Correlates for Social Exclusion and Self-Control. Society for Neuroscience, 1, 124-134. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470910601035160
[5]
Carp, J., & Compton, R. J. (2009). Alpha Power Is Influenced by Performance Errors. Psychophysiology, 46, 336-343. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00773.x
[6]
Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: An Open Source Toolbox for Analysis of Single-Trial EEG Dynamics including Independent Component Analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134, 9-21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009
[7]
Egner, T. (2011). Right Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex Mediates Individual Differences in Conflict-Driven Cognitive Control. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 3903-3913. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00064
[8]
Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does Rejection Hurt? An fMRI Study of Social Exclusion. Science, 302, 290-292. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1089134
[9]
Fishbach, A., Friedman, R. S., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2003). Leading Us Not unto Temptation: Momentary Allurements Elicit Overriding Goal Activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 296-309. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.296
[10]
Folstein, J. R., & Van Petten, C. (2008). Influence of Cognitive Control and Mismatch on the N2 Component of the ERP: A Review. Psychophysiology, 45, 152-170.
[11]
Fruhholz, S., Godde, B., Finke, M., & Herrmann, M. (2011). Spatio-Temporal Brain Dynamics in a Combined Stimulus-Stimulus and Stimulus-Response Conflict Task. Neuroimage, 54, 622-634. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.071
[12]
Gerber, J., & Wheeler, L. (2009). On Being Rejected a Meta-Analysis of Experimental Research on Rejection. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 468-488. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01158.x
[13]
Jamieson, J. P., Harkins, S. G., & Williams, K. D. (2010). Need Threat Can Motivate Performance after Ostracism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 690-702. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167209358882
[14]
Kelly, S. P., Lalor, E. C., Reilly, R. B., & Foxe, J. J. (2006). Increases in Alpha Oscillatory Power Reflect an Active Retinotopic Mechanism for Distracter Suppression during Sustained Visuospatial Attention. Journal of Neurophysiology, 95, 3844-3851. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01234.2005
[15]
Liu, P. D., Yang, W. J., Chen, J., Huang, X. T., & Chen, A. T. (2013). Alertness Modulates Conflict Adaptation and Feature Integration in an Opposite Way. PLoS ONE, 8, e79146. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0079146
Nee, D. E., Wager, T. D., & Jonides, J. (2007). Interference Resolution: Insights from a Meta-Analysis of Neuroimaging Tasks. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 1-17. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.7.1.1
[18]
Otten, M., & Jonas, K. J. (2013). Out of the Group, Out of Control? The Brain Responds to Social Exclusion with Changes in Cognitive Control. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 789-794. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss071
[19]
Pandey, A. K., Kamarajan, C., Manz, N., Chorlian, D. B., Stimus, A., & Porjesz, B. (2016). Delta, Theta, and Alpha Event-Related Oscillations in Alcoholics during Go/NoGo Task: Neurocognitive Deficits in Execution, Inhibition, and Attention Processing. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 65, 158-171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2015.10.002
[20]
Peake, S. J., Dishion, T. J., Stormshak, E. A., Moore, W. E., & Pfeifer, J. H. (2013). Risk-Taking and Social Exclusion in Adolescence: Neural Mechanisms Underlying Peer Influences on Decision-Making. Neuroimage, 82, 23-34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.061
[21]
Qian, M., Wu, G., Zhu, R., & Zhang, S. (2000). Development of the Revised Eysenck Personality Uestionnaire Short Scale for Chinese (EPQ-RSC). Acta Psychologica Sinica, 32, 317-323.
[22]
Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Stadler, W., Schabus, M., Doppelmayr, M., Hanslmayr, S., Birbaumer, N. et al. (2005). A Shift of Visual Spatial Attention Is Selectively Associated with Human EEG Alpha Activity. European Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 2917-2926. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04482.x
[23]
Schaafsma, J., Krahmer, E., Postma, M., Swerts, M., Balsters, M., & Vingerhoets, A. (2015). Comfortably Numb? Nonverbal Reactions to Social Exclusion. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 39, 25-39. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-014-0198-9
[24]
Twenge, J. M., Baumeister, R. F., Tice, D. M., & Stucke, T. S. (2001). If You Can’t Join Them, Beat Them: Effects of Social Exclusion on Aggressive Behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1058-1069. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.1058
[25]
Twenge, J. M., Catanese, K. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2002). Social Exclusion Causes Self-Defeating Behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 606-615. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.3.606
[26]
Twenge, J. M., Catanese, K. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2003). Social Exclusion and the Deconstructed State: Time Perception, Meaninglessness, Lethargy, Lack of Emotion, and Self-Awareness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 409-423. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.409
[27]
van Ede, F., de Lange, F., Jensen, O., & Maris, E. (2011). Orienting Attention to an Upcoming Tactile Event Involves a Spatially and Temporally Specific Modulation of Sensorimotor Alpha- and Beta-Band Oscillations. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 2016-2024. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5630-10.2011
[28]
van Veen, V., & Carter, C. S. (2002). The Timing of Action-Monitoring Processes in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 593-602. https://doi.org/10.1162/08989290260045837
[29]
Wang, L., Tang, D., Zhao, Y., Hitchman, G., Wu, S., Tan, J., & Chen, A. (2015). Disentangling the Impacts of Outcome Valence and Outcome Frequency on the Post-Error Slowing. Scientific Reports, 5, Article No. 8708. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep08708
[30]
Wang, T., Chen, Z. C., Zhao, G., Hitchman, G., Liu, C. C., Zhao, X. Y., Chen, A. T. et al. (2014). Linking Inter-Individual Differences in the Conflict Adaptation Effect to Spontaneous Brain Activity. Neuroimage, 90, 146-152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.055
[31]
Warburton, W. A., Williams, K. D., & Cairns, D. R. (2006). When Ostracism Leads to Aggression: The Moderating Effects of Control Deprivation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 213-220. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2005.03.005
[32]
Yanaka, H. T., Saito, D. N., Uchiyama, Y., & Sadato, N. (2010). Neural Substrates of Phasic Alertness: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study. Neuroscience Research, 68, 51-58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2010.05.005
[33]
Yeung, N., Botvinick, M. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The Neural Basis of Error Detection: Conflict Monitoring and the Error-Related Negativity. Psychological Review, 111, 931-959. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.111.4.931
[34]
Yordanova, J. Y., Kolev, V. N., & Basar, E. (1998). EEG Theta and Frontal Alpha Oscillations during Auditory Processing Change with Aging. Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology, 108, 497-505. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-5597(98)00028-8