The basal ganglia play a central role in regulating the response selection abilities that are critical for mental flexibility. In neocortical areas, higher cardiorespiratory fitness levels are associated with increased gray matter volume, and these volumetric differences mediate enhanced cognitive performance in a variety of tasks. Here we examine whether cardiorespiratory fitness correlates with the volume of the subcortical nuclei that make up the basal ganglia and whether this relationship predicts cognitive flexibility in older adults. Structural MRI was used to determine the volume of the basal ganglia nuclei in a group of older, neurologically healthy individuals (mean age 66 years, ?? = 1 7 9 ). Measures of cardiorespiratory fitness ( V O 2 m a x ), cognitive flexibility (task switching), and attentional control (flanker task) were also collected. Higher fitness levels were correlated with higher accuracy rates in the Task Switching paradigm. In addition, the volume of the caudate nucleus, putamen, and globus pallidus positively correlated with Task Switching accuracy. Nested regression modeling revealed that caudate nucleus volume was a significant mediator of the relationship between cardiorespiratory fitness, and task switching performance. These findings indicate that higher cardiorespiratory fitness predicts better cognitive flexibility in older adults through greater grey matter volume in the dorsal striatum. 1. Introduction Age-related cognitive decline is an unfortunate, but nearly ubiquitous, characteristic of late life that is preceded by atrophy of several brain regions including the prefrontal cortex, medial temporal lobe, and basal ganglia [1, 2]. Because of the expected increase in the proportion of adults over the age of 65 in the next forty years, it has become a major public health initiative to identify methods to prevent or reverse regional brain atrophy with the hope that this might concurrently improve cognitive performance [3]. Randomized trials of aerobic exercise have proven promising from this regard, with participation in exercise programs leading to greater prefrontal [4] and hippocampal volumes [5]. Nonrandomized longitudinal studies of physical activity [6, 7] and cross-sectional studies of cardiorespiratory fitness [8–10] have shown similar results, with more physical activity and higher fitness levels associated with greater volumes. Unfortunately, few studies have examined whether cardiorespiratory fitness levels in older adult humans are associated with brain areas other than the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus
References
[1]
N. Raz, U. Lindenberger, K. M. Rodrigue et al., “Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and modifiers,” Cerebral Cortex, vol. 15, no. 11, pp. 1676–1689, 2005.
[2]
K. B. Walhovd, A. M. Fjell, A. M. Dale et al., “Multi-modal imaging predicts memory performance in normal aging and cognitive decline,” Neurobiology of Aging, vol. 31, no. 7, pp. 1107–1121, 2010.
[3]
S. C. Cramer, M. Sur, B. H. Dobkin et al., “Harnessing neuroplasticity for clinical applications,” Brain, vol. 134, no. 6, pp. 1591–1609, 2011.
[4]
S. J. Colcombe, K. I. Erickson, P. E. Scalf et al., “Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans,” Journals of Gerontology A, vol. 61, no. 11, pp. 1166–1170, 2006.
[5]
K. I. Erickson, M. W. Voss, R. S. Prakash et al., “Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 108, no. 7, pp. 3017–3022, 2011.
[6]
K. I. Erickson, C. A. Raji, O. L. Lopez et al., “Physical activity predicts gray matter volume in late adulthood: the Cardiovascular Health Study,” Neurology, vol. 75, no. 16, pp. 1415–1422, 2010.
[7]
S. Rovio, G. Spulber, L. J. Nieminen et al., “The effect of midlife physical activity on structural brain changes in the elderly,” Neurobiology of Aging, vol. 31, no. 11, pp. 1927–1936, 2010.
[8]
S. J. Colcombe, K. I. Erickson, N. Raz et al., “Aerobic fitness reduces brain tissue loss in aging humans,” Journals of Gerontology A, vol. 58, no. 2, pp. 176–180, 2003.
[9]
K. I. Erickson, R. S. Prakash, M. W. Voss et al., “Aerobic fitness is associated with hippocampal volume in elderly humans,” Hippocampus, vol. 19, no. 10, pp. 1030–1039, 2009.
[10]
A. M. Weinstein, M. W. Voss, R. S. Prakash et al., “The association between aerobic fitness and executive function is mediated by prefrontal cortex volume,” Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, vol. 2, no. 1, pp. 32–41, 2012.
[11]
A. M. Fjell and K. B. Walhovd, “Structural brain changes in aging: courses, causes and cognitive consequences,” Reviews in the Neurosciences, vol. 21, no. 3, pp. 187–221, 2010.
[12]
E. Aarts, M. Van Holstein, and R. Cools, “Striatal dopamine and the interface between motivation and cognition,” Frontiers in Psychology, vol. 2, p. 163, 2011.
[13]
R. Cools, L. Clark, and T. W. Robbins, “Differential responses in human striatum and prefrontal cortex to changes in object and rule relevance,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 24, no. 5, pp. 1129–1135, 2004.
[14]
L. Marais, D. J. Stein, and W. M. U. Daniels, “Exercise increases BDNF levels in the striatum and decreases depressive-like behavior in chronically stressed rats,” Metabolic Brain Disease, vol. 24, no. 4, pp. 587–597, 2009.
[15]
C. T. Wu, M. B. Pontifex, L. B. Raine et al., “Aerobic fitness and response variability in preadolescent children performing a cognitive control task,” Neuropsychology, vol. 25, no. 3, pp. 333–341, 2011.
[16]
M. J. Zigmond, J. L. Cameron, R. K. Leak et al., “Triggering endogenous neuroprotective processes through exercise in models of dopamine deficiency,” Parkinsonism and Related Disorders, vol. 15, no. 3, pp. S42–S45, 2009.
[17]
J. Y. F. Lau, D. Goldman, B. Buzas et al., “BDNF gene polymorphism (Val66Met) predicts amygdala and anterior hippocampus responses to emotional faces in anxious and depressed adolescents,” NeuroImage, vol. 53, no. 3, pp. 952–961, 2010.
[18]
N. Tajiri, T. Yasuhara, T. Shingo et al., “Exercise exerts neuroprotective effects on Parkinson's disease model of rats,” Brain Research, vol. 1310, pp. 200–207, 2010.
[19]
J. E. Ahlskog, “Does vigorous exercise have a neuroprotective effect in Parkinson disease?” Neurology, vol. 77, no. 3, pp. 288–294, 2011.
[20]
A. D. Speelman, M. van Nimwegen, G. F. Borm, B. R. Bloem, and M. Munneke, “Monitoring of walking in Parkinson's disease: validation of an ambulatory activity monitor,” Parkinsonism and Related Disorders, vol. 17, no. 5, pp. 402–404, 2011.
[21]
L. Chaddock, K. I. Erickson, R. S. Prakash et al., “Basal ganglia volume is associated with aerobic fitness in preadolescent children,” Developmental Neuroscience, vol. 32, no. 3, pp. 249–256, 2010.
[22]
A. A. Kehagia, R. A. Barker, and T. W. Robbins, “Neuropsychological and clinical heterogeneity of cognitive impairment and dementia in patients with Parkinson's disease,” The Lancet Neurology, vol. 9, no. 12, pp. 1200–1213, 2010.
[23]
C. Stelzel, U. Basten, C. Montag, M. Reuter, and C. J. Fiebach, “Frontostriatal involvement in task switching depends on genetic differences in D2 receptor density,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 30, no. 42, pp. 14205–14212, 2010.
[24]
Y. Stern, M. Sano, J. Paulsen, and R. Mayeux, “Modified mini-mental state examination: validity and reliability,” Neurology, vol. 37, p. 179, 1987.
[25]
E. McAuley, A. N. Szabo, E. L. Mailey et al., “Non-exercise estimated cardiorespiratory fitness: associations with brain structure, cognition, and memory complaints in older adults,” Mental Health and Physical Activity, vol. 4, no. 1, pp. 5–11, 2011.
[26]
R. S. Prakash, M. W. Voss, K. I. Erickson et al., “Cardiorespiratory fitness and attentional control in the aging brain,” Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol. 4, article 229, 2010.
[27]
M. W. Voss, K. I. Erickson, R. S. Prakash et al., “Functional connectivity: a source of variance in the association between cardiorespiratory fitness and cognition?” Neuropsychologia, vol. 48, no. 5, pp. 1394–1406, 2010.
[28]
J. I. Sheikh and J. A. Yesavage, “Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): recent evidence and development of a shorter version,” Clinical Gerontologist, vol. 5, no. 1-2, pp. 165–173, 1986.
[29]
B. Patenaude, S. M. Smith, D. N. Kennedy, and M. Jenkinson, “A Bayesian model of shape and appearance for subcortical brain segmentation,” NeuroImage, vol. 56, no. 3, pp. 907–922, 2011.
[30]
Y. Zhang, M. Brady, and S. Smith, “Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden Markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm,” IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, vol. 20, no. 1, pp. 45–57, 2001.
[31]
S. M. Smith, M. Jenkinson, M. W. Woolrich et al., “Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL,” NeuroImage, vol. 23, supplement 1, pp. S208–S219, 2004.
[32]
K. M. Kennedy, K. I. Erickson, K. M. Rodrigue et al., “Age-related differences in regional brain volumes: a comparison of optimized voxel-based morphometry to manual volumetry,” Neurobiology of Aging, vol. 30, no. 10, pp. 1657–1676, 2009.
[33]
A. Miyake, N. P. Friedman, M. J. Emerson, A. H. Witzki, A. Howerter, and T. D. Wager, “The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis,” Cognitive Psychology, vol. 41, no. 1, pp. 49–100, 2000.
[34]
K. J. Preacher and A. F. Hayes, “Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models,” Behavior Research Methods, vol. 40, no. 3, pp. 879–891, 2008.
[35]
A. F. Hayes, “Beyond Baron and Kenny: statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium,” Communication Monographs, vol. 76, no. 4, pp. 408–420, 2009.
[36]
S. J. Colcombe, A. F. Kramer, K. I. Erickson et al., “Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 101, no. 9, pp. 3316–3321, 2004.
[37]
A. F. Kramer, S. Hahn, N. J. Cohen et al., “Ageing, fitness and neurocognitive function,” Nature, vol. 400, no. 6743, pp. 418–419, 1999.
[38]
B. N. Greenwood, T. E. Foley, T. V. Le et al., “Long-term voluntary wheel running is rewarding and produces plasticity in the mesolimbic reward pathway,” Behavioural Brain Research, vol. 217, no. 2, pp. 354–362, 2011.
[39]
M. D. Roberts, L. Gilpin, K. E. Parker, T. E. Childs, M. J. Will, and F. W. Booth, “Dopamine D1 receptor modulation in nucleus accumbens lowers voluntary wheel running in rats bred to run high distances,” Physiology & Behavior, vol. 105, no. 3, pp. 661–668, 2012.
[40]
W. F. Mathes, D. L. Nehrenberg, R. Gordon, K. Hua, T. Garland Jr., and D. Pomp, “Dopaminergic dysregulation in mice selectively bred for excessive exercise or obesity,” Behavioural Brain Research, vol. 210, no. 2, pp. 155–163, 2010.
[41]
C. A. Fontes-Ribeiro, E. Marques, F. C. Pereira, A. P. Silva, and T. R. A. Macedo, “May exercise prevent addiction?” Current Neuropharmacology, vol. 9, no. 1, pp. 45–48, 2011.
[42]
S. J. O'Dell, B. A. Galvez, A. J. Ball, and J. F. Marshall, “Running wheel exercise ameliorates methamphetamine-induced damage to dopamine and serotonin terminals,” Synapse, vol. 66, pp. 71–80, 2012.
[43]
M. L. Mustroph, D. J. Stobaugh, D. S. Miller, E. K. DeYoung, and J. S. Rhodes, “Wheel running can accelerate or delay extinction of conditioned place preference for cocaine in male C57BL/6J mice, depending on timing of wheel access,” European Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 34, pp. 1161–1169, 2011.
[44]
N. Tajiri, T. Yasuhara, T. Shingo et al., “Exercise exerts neuroprotective effects on Parkinson's disease model of rats,” Brain Research, vol. 1310, pp. 200–207, 2010.
[45]
J. Rico-Sanz, T. Rankinen, T. Rice et al., “Quantitative trait loci for maximal exercise capacity phenotypes and their responses to training in the HERITAGE Family Study,” Physiological Genomics, vol. 16, pp. 256–260, 2004.
[46]
D. P. Casey, B. D. Madery, T. B. Curry, J. H. Eisenach, B. W. Wilkins, and M. J. Joyner, “Nitric oxide contributes to the augmented vasodilatation during hypoxic exercise,” Journal of Physiology, vol. 588, no. 2, pp. 373–385, 2010.
[47]
E. S. Anderson, “Morphology of early developing oligodendrocytes in the ventrolateral spinal cord of the chicken,” Journal of Neurocytology, vol. 32, no. 9, pp. 1045–1053, 2003.
[48]
E. Dahlin, A. S. Neely, A. Larsson, L. B?ckman, and L. Nyberg, “Transfer of learning after updating training mediated by the striatum,” Science, vol. 320, no. 5882, pp. 1510–1512, 2008.
[49]
L. Chaddock, K. I. Erickson, R. S. Prskash et al., “A functional MRI investigation of the association between childhood aerobic fitness and neurocognitive control,” " Biological Psychology, vol. 89, no. 1, pp. 260–268, 2012.
[50]
C. H. Hillman, S. M. Buck, J. R. Themanson, M. B. Pontifex, and D. M. Castelli, “Aerobic fitness and cognitive development: event-related brain potential and task performance indices of executive control in preadolescent children,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 45, no. 1, pp. 114–129, 2009.
[51]
M. B. Pontifex, M. R. Scudder, M. L. Brown et al., “On the number of trials necessary for stabilization of error-related brain activity across the life span,” Psychophysiology, vol. 47, no. 4, pp. 767–773, 2010.
[52]
J. R. Themanson, C. H. Hillman, E. McAuley et al., “Self-efficacy effects on neuroelectric and behavioral indices of action monitoring in older adults,” Neurobiology of Aging, vol. 29, no. 7, pp. 1111–1122, 2008.
[53]
S. Stroth, S. Kubesch, K. Dieterle, M. Ruchsow, R. Heim, and M. Kiefer, “Physical fitness, but not acute exercise modulates event-related potential indices for executive control in healthy adolescents,” Brain Research, vol. 1269, pp. 114–124, 2009.