全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Association of Social Engagement with Brain Volumes Assessed by Structural MRI

DOI: 10.1155/2012/512714

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

We tested the hypothesis that social engagement is associated with larger brain volumes in a cohort study of 348 older male former lead manufacturing workers ( ?? = 3 0 5 ) and population-based controls ( ?? = 4 3 ), age 48 to 82. Social engagement was measured using a summary scale derived from confirmatory factor analysis. The volumes of 20 regions of interest (ROIs), including total brain, total gray matter (GM), total white matter (WM), each of the four lobar GM and WM, and 9 smaller structures were derived from T1-weighted structural magnetic resonance images. Linear regression models adjusted for age, education, race/ethnicity, intracranial volume, hypertension, diabetes, and control (versus lead worker) status. Higher social engagement was associated with larger total brain and GM volumes, specifically temporal and occipital GM, but was not associated with WM volumes except for corpus callosum. A voxel-wise analysis supported an association in temporal lobe GM. Using longitudinal data to discern temporal relations, change in ROI volumes over five years showed null associations with current social engagement. Findings are consistent with the hypothesis that social engagement preserves brain tissue, and not consistent with the alternate hypothesis that persons with smaller or shrinking volumes become less socially engaged, though this scenario cannot be ruled out. 1. Introduction Social engagement, the performance of meaningful social roles for either leisure or productive activity, has been shown to be associated with better cognitive function and lowered rates of cognitive decline and dementia in older adults [1–4]. Yet many questions remain regarding how social engagement can potentially get “under the skull” to preserve cognitive abilities. Inconsistencies in measurement across studies is frequent with a number of overlapping constructs such as social activity [1, 5], social networks [6, 7], and social support [8] linked to cognitive outcomes; each has been theorized to affect the brain through separate mechanisms. Yet the neurological mechanisms that could lead to preservation of cognitive function remain unclear and perhaps the largest obstacle is a lack of research to directly explore the biological effects of social engagement on the brain. A popular hypothesis is that social engagement helps to build a brain reserve capacity that allows the brain to tolerate neuropathologic damage due to aging or disease without deterioration of cognitive abilities [9, 10]. In a case of “use it or lose it,” remaining socially engaged as one ages may build

References

[1]  B. D. James, R. S. Wilson, L. L. Barnes, and D. A. Bennett, “Late-life social activity and cognitive decline in old age,” Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, vol. 17, no. 6, pp. 998–1005, 2011.
[2]  K. A. Ertel, M. M. Glymour, and L. F. Berkman, “Effects of social integration on preserving memory function in a nationally representative US elderly population,” American Journal of Public Health, vol. 98, no. 7, pp. 1215–1220, 2008.
[3]  S. S. Bassuk, T. A. Glass, and L. F. Berkman, “Social disengagement and incident cognitive decline in community—dwelling elderly persons,” Annals of Internal Medicine, vol. 131, no. 3, pp. 165–173, 1999.
[4]  L. Fratiglioni, S. Paillard-Borg, and B. Winblad, “An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia,” Lancet Neurology, vol. 3, no. 6, pp. 343–353, 2004.
[5]  A. Karp, S. Paillard-Borg, H. X. Wang, M. Silverstein, B. Winblad, and L. Fratiglioni, “Mental, physical and social components in leisure activities equally contribute to decrease dementia risk,” Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, vol. 21, no. 2, pp. 65–73, 2006.
[6]  L. L. Barnes, C. F. Mendes De Leon, R. S. Wilson, J. L. Bienias, and D. A. Evans, “Social resources and cognitive decline in a population of older African Americans and whites,” Neurology, vol. 63, no. 12, pp. 2322–2326, 2004.
[7]  R. E. Holtzman, G. W. Rebok, J. S. Saczynski, A. C. Kouzis, K. W. Doyle, and W. W. Eaton, “Social network characteristics and cognition in middle-aged and older adults,” Journals of Gerontology B, vol. 59, no. 6, pp. P278–P284, 2004.
[8]  T. E. Seeman, T. M. Lusignolo, M. Albert, and L. Berkman, “Social relationships, social support, and patterns of cognitive aging in healthy, high-functioning older adults: MacArthur studies of successful aging,” Health Psychology, vol. 20, no. 4, pp. 243–255, 2001.
[9]  L. Fratiglioni and H. X. Wang, “Brain reserve hypothesis in dementia,” Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, vol. 12, no. 1, pp. 11–22, 2007.
[10]  M. J. Valenzuela and P. Sachdev, “Brain reserve and cognitive decline: a non-parametric systematic review,” Psychological Medicine, vol. 36, no. 8, pp. 1065–1073, 2006.
[11]  G. Kempermann, D. Gast, and F. H. Gage, “Neuroplasticity in old age: Sustained fivefold induction of hippocampal neurogenesis by long-term environmental enrichment,” Annals of Neurology, vol. 52, no. 2, pp. 135–143, 2002.
[12]  N. W. Milgram, C. T. Siwak-Tapp, J. Araujo, and E. Head, “Neuroprotective effects of cognitive enrichment,” Ageing Research Reviews, vol. 5, no. 3, pp. 354–369, 2006.
[13]  M. C. Diamond, “Response of the brain to enrichment,” Anais da Academia Brasileira de Ciências, vol. 73, pp. 211–220, 2001.
[14]  G. Kempermann, H. G. Kuhn, and F. H. Gage, “More hippocampal neurons in adult mice living in an enriched environment,” Nature, vol. 386, no. 6624, pp. 493–495, 1997.
[15]  B. S. Schwartz, S. Chen, B. Caffo et al., “Relations of brain volumes with cognitive function in males 45 years and older with past lead exposure,” NeuroImage, vol. 37, no. 2, pp. 633–641, 2007.
[16]  N. T. Aggarwal, R. S. Wilson, J. L. Bienias et al., “The association of magnetic resonance imaging measures with cognitive function in a biracial population sample,” Archives of Neurology, vol. 67, no. 4, pp. 475–482, 2010.
[17]  R. Katzman, R. Terry, R. DeTeresa et al., “Clinical, pathological, and neurochemical changes in dementia: a subgroup with preserved mental status and numerous neocortical plaques,” Annals of Neurology, vol. 23, no. 2, pp. 138–144, 1988.
[18]  B. S. Schwartz, B. Caffo, W. F. Stewart et al., “Evaluation of cumulative lead dose and longitudinal changes in structural magnetic resonance imaging in former organolead workers,” Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, vol. 52, no. 4, pp. 407–414, 2010.
[19]  B. S. Schwartz, K. I. Bolla, W. Stewart, D. P. Ford, J. Agnew, and H. Frumkin, “Decrements in neurobehavioral performance associated with mixed exposure to organic and inorganic lead,” American Journal of Epidemiology, vol. 137, no. 9, pp. 1006–1021, 1993.
[20]  B. S. Schwartz, W. F. Stewart, K. I. Bolla et al., “Past adult lead exposure is associated with longitudinal decline in cognitive function,” Neurology, vol. 55, no. 8, pp. 1144–1150, 2000.
[21]  W. F. Stewart, B. S. Schwartz, D. Simon, K. I. Bolla, A. C. Todd, and J. Links, “Neurobehavioral function and tibial and chelatable lead levels in 543 former organolead workers,” Neurology, vol. 52, no. 8, pp. 1610–1617, 1999.
[22]  W. F. Stewart, B. S. Schwartz, C. Davatzikos et al., “Past adult lead exposure is linked to neurodegeneration measured by brain MRI,” Neurology, vol. 66, no. 10, pp. 1476–1484, 2006.
[23]  A. F. Goldszal, C. Davatzikos, D. L. Pham, M. X. H. Yan, R. N. Bryan, and S. M. Resnick, “An image-processing system for qualitative and quantitative volumetric analysis of brain images,” Journal of Computer Assisted Tomography, vol. 22, no. 5, pp. 827–837, 1998.
[24]  Z. Xue, D. Shen, and C. Davatzikos, “CLASSIC: consistent longitudinal alignment and segmentation for serial image computing,” NeuroImage, vol. 30, no. 2, pp. 388–399, 2006.
[25]  D. Shen and C. Davatzikos, “HAMMER: Hierarchical attribute matching mechanism for elastic registration,” IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, vol. 21, no. 11, pp. 1421–1439, 2002.
[26]  T. A. Glass, “Conjugating the “tenses” of function: discordance among hypothetical, experimental, and enacted function in older adults,” Gerontologist, vol. 38, no. 1, pp. 101–112, 1998.
[27]  S. M. Resnick, D. L. Pham, M. A. Kraut, A. B. Zonderman, and C. Davatzikos, “Longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging studies of older adults: a shrinking brain,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 23, no. 8, pp. 3295–3301, 2003.
[28]  C. D. Smith, H. Chebrolu, D. R. Wekstein, F. A. Schmitt, and W. R. Markesbery, “Age and gender effects on human brain anatomy: a voxel-based morphometric study in healthy elderly,” Neurobiology of Aging, vol. 28, no. 7, pp. 1075–1087, 2007.
[29]  R. D. Terry, R. DeTeresa, and L. A. Hansen, “Neocortical cell counts in normal human adult aging,” Annals of Neurology, vol. 21, no. 6, pp. 530–539, 1987.
[30]  E. Masliah, M. Mallory, L. Hansen, R. DeTeresa, and R. D. Terry, “Quantitative synaptic alterations in the human neocortex during normal aging,” Neurology, vol. 43, no. 1, pp. 192–197, 1993.
[31]  G. L. Wenk, “Neuropathologic changes in Alzheimer's disease,” Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, vol. 64, supplement 9, pp. 7–10, 2003.
[32]  N. Scarmeas and Y. Stern, “Cognitive reserve and lifestyle,” Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, vol. 25, no. 5, pp. 625–633, 2003.
[33]  B. Draganski and A. May, “Training-induced structural changes in the adult human brain,” Behavioural Brain Research, vol. 192, no. 1, pp. 137–142, 2008.
[34]  E. Gould, A. J. Reeves, M. S. A. Graziano, and C. G. Gross, “Neurogenesis in the neocortex of adult primates,” Science, vol. 286, no. 5439, pp. 548–552, 1999.
[35]  H. Van Praag, G. Kempermann, and F. H. Gage, “Neural Consequences of environmental enrichment,” Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 1, no. 3, pp. 191–198, 2000.
[36]  C. T. Siwak-Tapp, E. Head, B. A. Muggenburg, N. W. Milgram, and C. W. Cotman, “Region specific neuron loss in the aged canine hippocampus is reduced by enrichment,” Neurobiology of Aging, vol. 29, no. 1, pp. 39–50, 2008.
[37]  E. R. Kandel, J. H. Schwartz, and T. M. Jessell, Principles of Neural Science, Appleton & Lange, East Norwalk, Conn, USA, 3rd edition, 1991.
[38]  T. L. Jernigan, S. L. Archibald, C. Fennema-Notestine et al., “Effects of age on tissues and regions of the cerebrum and cerebellum,” Neurobiology of Aging, vol. 22, no. 4, pp. 581–594, 2001.
[39]  R. I. Scahill, C. Frost, R. Jenkins, J. L. Whitwell, M. N. Rossor, and N. C. Fox, “A longitudinal study of brain volume changes in normal aging using serial registered magnetic resonance imaging,” Archives of Neurology, vol. 60, no. 7, pp. 989–994, 2003.
[40]  C. D. Good, I. S. Johnsrude, J. Ashburner, R. N. A. Henson, K. J. Friston, and R. S. J. Frackowiak, “A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains,” NeuroImage, vol. 14, no. 1 I, pp. 21–36, 2001.
[41]  J. S. Saczynski, L. A. Pfeifer, K. Masaki, et al., “The effect of social engagement on incident dementia and hippocampal volume: the Honolulu-Asia aging study,” Alzheimer's & Dementia, vol. 1, no. 1, supplement, p. S27, 2005.
[42]  R. Cabeza, N. D. Anderson, J. K. Locantore, and A. R. McIntosh, “Aging gracefully: compensatory brain activity in high-performing older adults,” NeuroImage, vol. 17, no. 3, pp. 1394–1402, 2002.
[43]  D. W. Desmond, J. T. Moroney, M. Sano, and Y. Stern, “Mortality in patients with dementia after ischemic stroke,” Neurology, vol. 59, no. 4, pp. 537–543, 2002.

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133