We administrate the cognitive reflection test devised by Frederick to a
sample of 483 undergraduates and discriminate the sample to consider selected
demographic characteristics. For the sake of robustness, we take two extra
versions that present cues for removing the automatic (but wrong) answers
suggested by the test. We find a participant’s gender and religious attitude to
matter for the test performance on the three versions. Males score
significantly higher than females, and so do atheists of either gender. While
the former result replicates a previous finding that is now reasonably well
established, the latter is new. The fact that atheists score higher agrees with
the literature showing that belief is an automatic manifestation of the mind
and its default mode. Disbelieving seems to require deliberative cognitive
ability. Such results are verified by an extra sample of 81 participants using
Google Docs questionnaires via the Internet.
Kahneman, D.
(2011) Thinking, Fast and Slow. Farrar, Straus and Giroux, New
York.
[4]
Stanovich, K.E.
and West, R.F. (2000) Individual Differences in Reasoning:
Implications for the Rationality Debate. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23,
645-665. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00003435
Trivers, R.
(2011) The Folly of Fools: The Logic of Deceit and Self-Deception in Human
Life. Basic Books, New York.
[7]
Wason, P.
(1966) Reasoning. In: Foss, B.M., Ed., New Horizons in Psychology, Vol.
1, Penguin, Harmondsworth, 135-151.
[8]
Cosmides, L.
and Tooby, J. (1992) Cognitive Adaptations for Social
Exchange. In:
Barkow, J.H., Cosmides, L.,
Tooby, J., Eds., The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture, Oxford University Press, Oxford,
163-228.
Houde, O.,
Zago, L.,
Crivello, F.,
Moutier, S.,
Pineau, A.,
Mazoyer, B. and Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2001) Access to Deductive Logic Depends upon a
Right Ventromedial Prefrontal Area Devoted to Emotion and Feeling: Evidence
from a Training Paradigm. Neuroimage, 14, 1486-1492. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0930
[11]
Houde, O.,
Zago, L.,
Mellet, E.,
Moutier, S.,
Pineau, A.,
Mazoyer, B. and Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2000) Shifting from the Perceptual Brain to
the Logical Brain: The Neural Impact of Cognitive Inhibition Training. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12,
721-728. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089892900562525
[12]
Goel, V.,
Buchel, C.,
Frith, C. and Dolan, R.J.
(2000) Dissociation of Mechanisms Underlying Syllogistic Reasoning. Neuroimage, 12,
504-514. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2000.0636
[13]
Mithen, S.
(2002) Human Evolution and the Cognitive Basis of Science. In: Carruthers, P., Stich, S. and Siegel, M., The
Cognitive Basis of Science, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 23-40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511613517.003
Toplak, M.E.,
West, R.F. and Stanovich, K.E. (2011) The Cognitive Reflection Test as a
Predictor of Performance on Heuristics-and-Biases Tasks. Memory & Cognition, 39,
1275-1289. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13421-011-0104-1
[16]
Heider, F. and Simmel, M.
(1944) An Experimental
Study of Apparent Behavior. The American
Journal of Psychology,57, 243-259. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1416950
[17]
Leslie, A.M. and Keeble, S. (1987)
Do Six-Month-Old Infants Perceive Causality? Cognition, 25, 265-288.
[18]
Bloom, P.
(2005) Is God an Accident? The Atlantic, 12,
105-112.
Kirkpatrick, L.A. and Epstein, S.
(1992) Cognitive-Experiential
Self-Theory and Subjective Probability: Further Evidence for Two Conceptual
Systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63,
534-544. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.534