全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...
PLOS ONE  2013 

Event-Related Brain Potentials for Goal-Related Power Grips

DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068501

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Recent research has shown that neurophysiological activation during action planning depends on the orientation to initial or final action goals for precision grips. However, the neural signature for a distinct class of grasping, power grips, is still unknown. The aim of the present study was to differentiate between cerebral activity, by means of event-related potentials (ERPs), and its temporal organization during power grips executed with an emphasis on either the initial or final parts of movement sequences. In a grasp and transportation task, visual cues emphasized either the grip (the immediate goal) or the target location (the final goal). ERPs differed between immediate and final goal-cued conditions, suggesting different means of operation dependent on goal-relatedness. Differences in mean amplitude occurred earlier for power grips than for recently reported precision grips time-locked to grasping over parieto-occipital areas. Time-locked to final object placement, differences occurred within a similar time window for power and precision grips over frontal areas. These results suggest that a parieto-frontal network of activation is of crucial importance for grasp planning and execution. Our results indicate that power grip preparation and execution for goal-related actions are controlled by similar neural mechanisms as have been observed during precision grips, but with a distinct temporal pattern.

References

[1]  Napier JR (1956) The prehensile movements of the human hand. J Bone Joint Surg 38 B(4): 902–913.
[2]  Schack T, Ritter H (2009) The cognitive nature of action - functional links between cognitive psychology, movement science and robotics. Prog Brain Res 174: 231–252.
[3]  Woodworth RS (1899) The accuracy of voluntary movement. Psychol Rev 3 (3 Suppl. 13)1–119.
[4]  Elliott D, Helsen WF, Chua R (2001) A century later: Woodworth’s (1899) two-component model of goal-directed aiming. Psychol Bull 127(3): 342–357.
[5]  Rosenbaum D A, Marchak F, Barnes H J, Vaughan J, Slotta J, et al.. (1990) Constraints for action selection: Overhand versus underhand grips. In: Jeannerod M, editor. Attention and performance XIII. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 321–342.
[6]  Schütz C, Weigelt M, Odekerken D, Klein-Soetebier T, Schack T (2011) Motor control strategies in a continuous task space. Motor Control 15(3): 321–341.
[7]  Ansuini C, Glosa L, Turella L, Altoé G, Castiello U (2008) An object for an action, the same object for other actions: Effects on hand shaping. Exp Brain Res 185(1): 111–119.
[8]  Hughes CML, Seegelke C, Spiegel MA, Oehmichen C, Hammes J, et al. (2012) Corrections in Grasp Posture in Response to Modifications of Action Goals. PLOS ONE 7(9): e43015.
[9]  Castiello U, Bennett KMB, Paulignan Y (1992) Does the type of prehension influence the kinematics of reaching? Behav Brain Res 50(1–2): 7–15.
[10]  Jeannerod M (2001) Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition. Neuroimage 14(1 Pt2): 103–109.
[11]  Majdandzic J, Grol MJ, van Schie HT, Verhagen L, Toni I, et al. (2007) The role of immediate and final goals in action planning: An fMRI study. Neuroimage 37(2): 589–598.
[12]  Castiello U, Begliomini C (2008) The cortical control of visually guided grasping. Neuroscientist 14(2): 157–170.
[13]  Filimon F (2010) Human cortical control of hand movements: Parietofrontal networks for reaching, grasping, and pointing. Neuroscientist 16(4): 388–407.
[14]  Bozzacchi C, Giusti MA, Pitzalis S, Spinelli D, Di Russo F (2012) Awareness affects motor planning for goal-oriented actions. Biol Psychol 89(2): 503–514.
[15]  Kornhuber HH, Deecke L (1965) Hirnpotential?nderungen bei Willkürbewegungen und passiven Bewegungen des Menschen: Bereitschaftspotential und reafferente Potentiale. Pflugers Arch 284: 1–17.
[16]  Shibasaki H, Hallett M (2006) What is the Bereitschaftspotential? Clin Neurophysiol 117(11): 2341–2356.
[17]  Bozzacchi C, Giusti MA, Pitzalis S, Spinelli D, Di Russo F (2012) Similar cerebral motor plans for real and virtual actions. PLOS ONE 7(10): e47783.
[18]  Gratton G, Coles MGH, Sirevaag EJ, Eriksen CW, Donchin E (1988) Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: A psychophysiological analysis. J Exp Psychol 14(3): 331–344.
[19]  Van Schie HT, Bekkering H (2007) Neural mechanisms underlying immediate and final action goals in object use reflected by slow wave brain potentials. Brain Res 1148: 183–197.
[20]  Van Elk M, Bousardt R, Bekkering H, Van Schie HT (2012) Using goal- and grip-related information for understanding the correctness of other’s actions: An ERP study. PLOS ONE 7(5): e36450.
[21]  Botwinick J, Thompson LW (1966) Premotor and motor components of reaction time. J Exp Psychol 71(1): 9–15.
[22]  Elliott D, Hansen S, Grierson LEM, Lyons J, Bennett SJ, et al. (2010) Goal-directed aiming: Two components but multiple processes. Psychol Bull 136(6): 1023–1044.
[23]  Rosenbaum DA, Jorgensen MJ (1992) Planning macroscopic aspects of manual control. Hum Mov Sci 11(1–2): 61–69.
[24]  Marteniuk RG, MacKenzie CL, Jeannerod M, Athenes S, Dugas C (1987) Constraints on human arm movements. Can J Psychol 41(3): 365–378.
[25]  Kutas M, Donchin E (1974) Studies of squeezing: Handedness, responding hand, response force, and asymmetry of readiness potential. Science 186(4163): 545–548.
[26]  Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9(1): 97–113.
[27]  Oostenveld R, Praamstra P (2001) The five percent electrode system for high-resolution EEG and ERP measurements. Clin Neurophysiol 112(4): 713–719.
[28]  Gratton G, Coles MGH, Donchin E (1983) A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 55(4): 468–484.
[29]  Koester D, Schiller NO (2008) Morphological priming in overt language production: Electrophysiological evidence from Dutch. NeuroImage 42(4): 1622–1630.
[30]  Ganushchak LY, Christoffels IK, Schiller NO (2011) The use of electroencephalography in language production research: A review. Frontiers in Psychology 2(208): 1–6.
[31]  Kirsch W, Hennighausen E, R?sler F (2010) ERP correlates of linear hand movements in a motor reproduction task. Psychophysiology 47(3): 486–500.
[32]  Lange JJ, Wijers AA, Mulder LJ, Mulder G (1999) ERP effects of spatial attention and display search with unilateral and bilateral stimulus displays. Biol Psychol 50(3): 203–233.
[33]  Begliomini C, Wall MB, Smith AT, Castiello U (2007) Differential cortical activity for precision and whole-hand visually guided grasping in humans. Eur J Neurosci 25(4): 1245–1252.
[34]  Uithol S, Van Rooij I, Bekkering H, Haselager P (2012) Hierarchies in action and motor control. J Cogn Neurosci 24(5): 1077–1086.
[35]  Mordkoff JT, Miller J, Roch AC (1996) Absence of coactivation in the motor component: Evidence from psychophysiological measures of target detection. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 22(1): 25–41.
[36]  Miller JO, Low K (2001) Motor processes in simple, go/no-go, and choice reaction time tasks: A psychophysiological analysis. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 27(2): 266–289.
[37]  Danek RH, Mordkoff JT (2011) Unequal motor durations under simple-, go/no-go, and choice-RT tasks: Extension of Miller and Low (2001). J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 37(4): 1323–1329.
[38]  Lindemann O, Stenneken P, Van Schie HT, Bekkering H (2006) Semantic activation in action planning. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 32(3): 633–643.
[39]  Spiegel MA, Koester D, Weigelt M, Schack T (2012) The costs of changing an intended action: Movement planning, but not execution interferes with verbal working memory. Neurosci Lett 509(2): 82–86.
[40]  Hughes CML, Seegelke C, Rei?ig P, Schütz C (2012) Effects of stimulus cueing on bimanual grasp posture planning. Exp Brain Res 219(3): 391–401.

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133