全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...
Sensors  2011 

Cognitive Facilitation Following Intentional Odor Exposure

DOI: 10.3390/s110505469

Keywords: odors, cognitive facilitation, memory, alertness, essential oils

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

This paper reviews evidence that, in addition to incidental olfactory pollutants, intentional odor delivery can impact cognitive operations both positively and negatively. Evidence for cognitive facilitation/interference is reviewed alongside four potential explanations for odor-induced effects. It is concluded that the pharmacological properties of odors can induce changes in cognition. However, these effects can be accentuated/attenuated by the shift in mood following odor exposure, expectancy of cognitive effects, and cues to behavior via the contextual association with the odor. It is proposed that greater consideration is required in the intentional utilization of odors within both industrial and private locations, since differential effects are observed for odors with positive hedonic qualities.

References

[1]  Kim, K-H. Experimental demonstration of masking phenomenon between competing odorants via an air dilution sensory test. Sensors 2010, 10, 7287–7302.
[2]  Kim, K-H. The averaging effect of odorant mixing via air dilution sensory test: A case study on reduced sulfur compounds. Sensors 2011, 11, 1405–1417.
[3]  Kim, K-H; Park, S-Y. A comparative analysis of malodor samples between direct (olfactometry) and indirect (instrumental) methods. Atmos. Environ 2008, 42, 5061–5070.
[4]  Rotton, J. Affective and cognitive consequences of malodorous pollution. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol 1983, 4, 171–191.
[5]  Moss, M; Cook, J; Wesnes, K; Duckett, P. Aromas of rosemary and lavender essential oils differentially affect cognition and mood in healthy adults. Int. J. Neurosci 2003, 113, 15–38.
[6]  Moss, M; Hewitt, S; Moss, L; Wesnes, K. Modulation of cognitive performance and mood by aromas of peppermint and ylang-ylang. Int. J. Neurosci 2008, 118, 59–77.
[7]  Warm, JS; Dember, WN; Parasuraman, R. Effects of olfactory stimulation on performance and stress in a visual sustained attention task. J. Soc. Cosmet. Chem 1991, 42, 199–210.
[8]  Norrish, MI; Dwyer, KL. Preliminary investigation of the effect of peppermint oil on an objective measure of daytime sleepiness. Int. J. Psychophysiol 2005, 55, 291–298.
[9]  Moss, M; Howarth, R; Wilkinson, L; Wesnes, K. Expectancy and the aroma of Roman chamomile influence mood and cognition in healthy volunteers. IJCA 2006, 16, 63–73.
[10]  Gould, A; Martin, GN. A good odour to breathe? The effect of pleasant ambient odour on human visual vigilance. Appl. Cognit. Psychol 2001, 15, 225–232.
[11]  Marchand, S; Arsenault, P. Odors modulate pain perception. A gender specific effect. Physiol. Behav 2002, 76, 251–256.
[12]  Villemure, C; Slotnick, BM; Bushnell, MC. Effects of odours on pain perception: Deciphering the roles of emotion and attention. Pain 2003, 106, 101–108.
[13]  Roberts, SC; Little, AC; Lyndon, A; Roberts, J; Havlicek, J; Wright, L. Manipulation of body odour alters men’s self-confidence and judgements of their visual attractiveness by women. Int. J. Cosmet. Sci 2009, 31, 47–54.
[14]  Mitchell, DJ; Kahn, BE; Knasko, SC. There’s something in the air: Effects of congruent or incongruent ambient odor on consumer decision making. J. Consum. Res 1995, 22, 229–238.
[15]  Jellinek, JS. Psychodynamic odour effects and their mechanisms. Cosmet. Toiletries 1997, 112, 61–71.
[16]  Smith, SM; Vela, E. Environmental context-dependent memory: A review and meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bull. Rev 2001, 8, 203–220.
[17]  Jirovetz, J; Buchbauer, G; Jager, W; Raverdino, V; Nikiforov, A. Determination of lavender oil fragrance compounds in blood samples. Fresen. J. Anal. Chem 1990, 338, 922–923.
[18]  Herz, R. Aromatherapy facts and fictions: A scientific analysis of olfactory effects on mood, physiology and behaviour. Int. J. Neurosci 2009, 119, 263–290.
[19]  Toda, M; Morimoto, K. Effect of lavender aroma on salivary endocrinological stress markers. Arch. Oral Biol 2008, 53, 964–968.
[20]  Lehrner, J; Marwinski, G; Lehr, S; Johren, P; Deecke, L. Ambient odors of orange and lavender reduce anxiety and improve mood in a dental office. Physiol. Behav 2005, 86, 92–95.
[21]  Holmes, C; Hopkins, V; Hensford, C; MacLaughlin, V; Wilkinson, D; Rosenvinge, H. Lavender oil as a treatment for agitated behaviour in severe dementia: A placebo controlled study. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatr 2002, 17, 305–308.
[22]  Woelk, H; Schl?fke, S. A multi-center, double-blind, randomised study of the Lavender oil preparation Silexan in comparison to Lorazepam for generalized anxiety disorder. Phytomedicine 2010, 17, 94–99.
[23]  Heuberger, E; Redhammer, S; Buchbauer, G. Transdermal absorption of (?)-Linalool induces autonomic deactivation but has no impact on ratings of well-being in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology 2004, 29, 1925–1932.
[24]  Bradley, BF; Brown, SL; Chu, S; Lea, RW. Effects of orally administered lavender essential oil on responses to anxiety-provoking film clips. Hum. Psychopharmacol. Clin. Exp 2009, 24, 319–330.
[25]  Kuroda, K; Inoue, N; Ito, Y; Kubota, K; Sugimoto, A; Kakuda, T; Fushiki, T. Sedative effects of the jasmine tea odor and (R)-(?)-linalool, one of its major odor components, on autonomic nerve activity and mood states. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol 2005, 95, 107–114.
[26]  Lorig, TS; Huffman, E; DeMartino, A; DeMarco, J. The effects of low concentration odors on EEG activity and behavior. J. Psychophysiol 1991, 5, 69–77.
[27]  Isen, AM. Positive affect and decision making. In Handbook of Emotions; Lewis, M, Haviland-Jones, J, Eds.; Guilford: New York, NY, USA, 2000; pp. 417–435.
[28]  Bell, IR; Hardin, EE; Baldwin, CM; Schwartz, GE. Increased limbic system symptomatology and sensitizability of young adults with chemical and noise sensitivities. Environ. Res 1995, 70, 84–97.
[29]  Herz, RS; Eliassen, JC; Beland, SL; Souza, T. Neuroimaging evidence for the emotional potency of odor-evoked memory. Neuropsychologia 2003, 42, 371–378.
[30]  Soussignan, R; Schaal, B. Children’s facial responsiveness to odors: Influences of hedonic valence of odors, age, gender and social presence. Dev. Psychol 1996, 32, 367–379.
[31]  Soussignan, R; Schaal, B; Schmit, G; Nadel, J. Facial responsiveness to odours in normal and pervasively developmentally disordered children. Chem. Senses 1995, 20, 47–59.
[32]  Millot, J-L; Brand, G. Effects of pleasant and unpleasant ambient odors on human voice pitch. Neurosci. Lett 2001, 297, 61–63.
[33]  Murray, IR; Arnott, JL. Toward the simulation of emotion in synthetic speech: A review of the literature on human vocal emotion. J. Acoust. Soc. Am 1993, 93, 1097–1108.
[34]  Castellanos, KM; Hudson, JA; Haviland-Jones, J; Wilson, PJ. Does exposure to ambient odors influence the emotional content of memories? Am. J. Psychol 2010, 123, 269–279.
[35]  Miltner, W; Matjak, M; Braun, C; Diekmann, H; Brody, S. Emotional qualities of odors and their influence on the startle reflex in humans. Psychophysiology 1994, 31, 107–110.
[36]  Knasko, SC. Ambient odor’s effect on creativity, mood, and perceived health. Chem. Senses 1992, 17, 27–35.
[37]  Baron, RA. Environmentally induced positive affect: Its impact on self-efficacy, task performance, negotiations, and conflict. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol 1990, 23, 368–384.
[38]  Chebat, JC; Michon, R. Impact of ambient odors on mall shoppers? Emotions, cognitions and spending: A test of competitive causal theories. J. Bus. Res 2003, 56, 529–539.
[39]  Weber, ST; Heuberger, E. The impact of natural odors on affective states in humans. Chem. Senses 2008, 33, 441–447.
[40]  Brauchli, P; Rüegg, PB; Etzweiler, F; Zeier, H. Electrocortical and autonomic alteration by administration of a pleasant and unpleasant odor. Chem. Senses 1995, 20, 505–515.
[41]  Mandairon, N; Kinster, C. Odor perception and olfactory bulb plasticity in adult mammals. J. Neurophysiol 2009, 101, 2204–2209.
[42]  Heuberger, E; Hongratanaworaki, T; B?hm, C; Weber, R; Buchbauer, G. Effects of chiral fragrances on human autonomic nervous system parameters and self-evaluation. Chem. Senses 2001, 26, 281–292.
[43]  Burnett, KM; Solterbeck, LA; Strapp, CM. Scent and mood state following an anxiety-provoking task. Psychol. Rep 2004, 95, 707–722.
[44]  Baron, RA; Thomley, J. A whiff of reality. Positive affect as a potential mediator of the effects of pleasant fragrances on task performance and helping. Environ. Behav 1994, 26, 766–784.
[45]  Baron, RA; Bronfen, J. A whiff of reality: Empirical evidence concerning the effects of pleasant fragrances on work-related behavior. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol 1994, 13, 1179–1203.
[46]  Baron, RA; Kalsher, MJ. Effects of pleasant ambient fragrance on simulated driving performance. The sweet smell of safety? Environ. Behav 1998, 30, 535–552.
[47]  Villemure, C; Wassimi, S; Bennett, GJ; Shir, Y; Bushnell, MC. Unpleasant odors increase pain processing in a patient with neuropathic pain: Psychophysical and fMRI investigation. Pain 2006, 120, 213–220.
[48]  Martin, GN. The effect of exposure to odor on the perception of pain. Psychosom. Med 2006, 68, 613–616.
[49]  Ludvigson, HW; Rottman, R. Effects of ambient odours of lavender and cloves on cognition, memory, affect and mood. Chem. Senses 1989, 14, 525–536.
[50]  Raudenbush, B; Koon, J; Smith, J; Zoladz, P. Effects of odorant administration on objective and subjective measures of sleep quality, post-sleep mood and alertness, and cognitive performance. N. Am. J. Psychol 2003, 5, 181–192.
[51]  Killgore, WDS; McBride, SA; Killgore, DB; Balkin, TJ; Kamimori, GH. Baseline odor identification ability predicts degradation of psychomotor vigilance during 77 hours of sleep deprivation. Int. J. Neurosci 2008, 118, 1207–1225.
[52]  Gottfried, J; Zald, DH. On the scent of human olfactory orbitofrontal cortex: Meta analysis and comparison to non-human primates. Brain Res 2005, 50, 287–304.
[53]  Michael, GA; Jacquot, L; Millot, J-L; Brand, G. Ambient odors modulate visual attentional capture. Neurosci. Lett 2003, 352, 221–225.
[54]  Michael, GA; Jacquot, L; Millot, J-L; Brand, G. Ambient odors influence the amplitude and time course of visual distraction. Behav. Neurosci 2005, 119, 708–715.
[55]  Hopkins, C. Aromatherapy: Remedies for Everyday Aliments; Parallel Book: London, UK, 1991.
[56]  Wilhelm, B; Giedke, H; Lüdtke, H; Bittner, E; Hofmann, A; Wilhelm, H. Daytime variations in central nervous system activation measured by a pupillographic sleepiness test. J. Sleep Res 2001, 10, 1–7.
[57]  Badia, P; Wesensten, N; Lammers, W; Culpepper, J; Harsh, J. Responsiveness to olfactory stimulus presented in sleep. Physiol. Behav 1990, 48, 87–90.
[58]  Raudenbush, B; Grayhem, R; Sears, T; Wilson, I. Effects of peppermint and cinnamon odor administration on simulated driving alertness, mood and workload. N. Am. J. Psychol 2009, 11, 245–256.
[59]  Ilmberger, J; Heuberger, E; Mahrhoferm, C; Dessovic, H; Kowarik, D; Buchbauer, G. The influence of essential oils on human attention. 1: Alertness. Chem. Senses 2001, 26, 239–245.
[60]  Heuberger, E; Ilmberger, J. The influence of essential oils on human vigilance. Nat. Prod. Commun 2010, 5, 1441–1446.
[61]  McBride, SA; Johnson, RF; Merullo, DJ; Bartow, RE. Effects of periodic administration of odor or vibration on a 3-hr vigilance task. Percept Mot. Skills 2004, 98, 307–318.
[62]  Ho, C; Spence, C. Olfactory facilitation of dual-task performance. Neurosci. Lett 2005, 389, 35–40.
[63]  Torii, S. The brain, the mind, and fragrance. Frag. Forum 1987, 11, 7.
[64]  Diego, MA; Jones, NA; Field, T; Hernadez-Reif, M; Schanberg, S; Kuhn, C; McAdam, V; Galamaga, R; Galamaga, M. Aromatherapy positively affects mood, EEG, patterns of alertness and math computations. Int. J. Neurosci 1998, 96, 217–224.
[65]  Field, T; Diego, M; Hernandez-Reif, M; Cisneros, W; Feijo, L; Vera, Y; Gil, K; Grina, D; He, Q. Lavender fragrance cleansing gel effects on relaxation. Int. J. Neurosci 2005, 115, 207–222.
[66]  Lorig, TS; Herman, KB; Schwartz, GE; Cain, WS. EEG activity during administration of low concentration odors. Bull. Psychonomic Soc 1990, 28, 405–408.
[67]  Lis-Balchin, M; Hart, S. Studies on the mode of action of the essential oil of lavender (Lavandula angustifolia). Phytother. Res 1999, 13, 540–542.
[68]  Millot, J-L; Brand, G; Morand, N. Effects of ambient odors on reaction times in humans. Neurosci. Lett 2002, 322, 79–82.
[69]  Sakamoto, R; Minoura, K; Usui, A; Ishizuka, Y; Kanba, S. Effectiveness of aroma on work efficiency: Lavender aroma during recesses prevents deterioration of work performance. Chem. Senses 2005, 30, 683–691.
[70]  Lombion, S; Comte, A; Tatu, L; Brand, G; Moulin, T; Millot, J-L. Patterns of cerebral activation during olfactory and trigeminal stimulations. Hum. Brain Mapp 2008, 30, 821–828.
[71]  Wilson, D; Stevenson, RJ. Learning to Smell; John Hopkins/University Press: Baltimore, MD, USA, 2006.
[72]  Howard, S; Hughes, BM. Expectancies, not aroma, explain impact of lavender aromatherapy on psychophysiological indices of relaxation in young healthy women. Br. J. Health Psychol 2008, 13, 603–617.
[73]  Sumner, D. On testing the sense of smell. Lancet 1962, 2, 895–897.
[74]  Kirsch, I. Are drug and placebo effects in depression additive? Biol. Psychiat 2000, 47, 733–735.
[75]  Knasko, S; Gilbert, A; Sabini, J. Emotional state, physical well-being, and performance in the presence of feigned ambient odor. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol 1990, 20, 1345–1357.
[76]  Campenni, CE; Crawley, EJ; Meier, ME. Role of suggestion in odor-induced mood change. Psychol. Rep 2004, 94, 1127–1136.
[77]  Engen, T. Odor memory. In Odor Sensation and Memory; Praeger: New York, NY, USA, 1991; pp. 77–87.
[78]  Chu, S; Downes, JJ. Long live Proust: The odour-cued autobiographical memory bump. Cognition 2000, 75, B41–B50.
[79]  Schoenbaum, G; Eichenbaum, H. Information coding in the rodent prefrontal cortex. Single-neuron activity in orbitofrontal cortex compared with that in pyriform cortex. J. Neurophysiol 1995, 74, 733–750.
[80]  Critchley, HD; Rolls, ET. Olfactory neuronal responses in the primate orbitofrontal cortex: Analysis in an olfactory discrimination task. J. Neurophysiol 1996, 75, 1659–1672.
[81]  Ramus, SJ; Eichenbaum, H. Neural correlates of olfactory recognition in the rat orbitofrontal cortex. J. Neurosci 2000, 20, 8199–8208.
[82]  Araujo, IE; Rolls, ET; Velazco, MI; Margot, C; Cayeux, I. Cognitive modulation of olfactory processing. Neuron 2005, 46, 671–679.
[83]  Davis, RG; Pangborn, RM. Odor pleasantness judgments compared among samples from 20 nations using microfragrance. Chem. Senses 1985, 30, 413.
[84]  Schaal, B; Soussignan, R; Marlier, L; Kontar, F; Karimal, IS; Tremblay, RE. Variability and invariants in early odor preferences: Comparative data from children belonging to three cultures. Chem. Senses 1997, 22, 212.
[85]  Wysocki, CJ; Pierce, JD; Gilbert, AN. Geographic, cross-cultural, and individual variations in human olfaction. In Smell and Taste in Health and Disease; Getchell, TV, Bartoshuk, LM, Doty, RL, Snow, JB, Eds.; Raven Press: New York, NY, USA, 1991; pp. 287–314.
[86]  Ayabe-Kanamura, S; Schicker, I; Laska, M; Hudson, R; Distel, R; Kobayakawa, T; Saito, S. Difference in perception of everyday odors: A Japanese-German cross cultural study. Chem. Senses 1998, 23, 31–38.
[87]  Cain, WS. To know with the nose: Keys to odor and identification. Science 1979, 203, 468–470.
[88]  Godden, DR; Baddeley, AD. Context dependent memory in two natural environments: On land and under water. Br. J. Psychol 1975, 66, 325–332.
[89]  Aggleton, JP; Waskett, L. The ability of odours to serve as state-dependent cues for real world memories: Can Viking smells aid the recall of Viking experiences? Br. J. Psychol 1999, 9, 1–17.
[90]  Cann, A; Ross, DA. Olfactory stimuli as context cues in human memory. Am. J. Psychol 1989, 102, 91–102.
[91]  Schab, FR. Odors and the remembrance of things past. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn 1990, 16, 648–655.
[92]  Ehrlichman, H; Halpern, JN. Affect and memory: Effects of pleasant and unpleasant odors on retrieval of happy and unhappy memories. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol 1988, 55, 769–779.
[93]  Chupnik, GC; Hilscher, MC; Buttu, D. Recognizing odors associated with meaningful places. Am. J. Psychol 2010, 123, 281–293.
[94]  Ball, PA; Shoker, J; Miles, JNV. Odour-based context reinstatement effects with indirect measures of memory: The curious case of rosemary. Br. J. Psychol 2010, 101, 655–678.
[95]  Robin, O; Alaoui-Ismaili, O; Dittmar, A; Vernet-Mauri, E. Emotional responses evoked by dental odors: An evaluation from autonomic parameters. J. Dent. Res 1998, 77, 1638–1646.
[96]  Epple, G; Herz, RS. Ambient odors associated to failure influence cognitive performance in children. Dev. Psychobiol 1999, 35, 103–107.
[97]  Herz, RS; Schankler, C; Beland, S. Olfaction, emotion and associative learning: Effects on motivated behavior. Motiv. Emot 2004, 28, 363–383.
[98]  Holland, RW; Hendriks, M; Aarts, H. Smells like clean spirit. Psychol. Sci 2005, 16, 689–693.

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133