全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...
PLOS ONE  2012 

CNTNAP2 and Language Processing in Healthy Individuals as Measured with ERPs

DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046995

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

The genetic FOXP2-CNTNAP2 pathway has been shown to be involved in the language capacity. We investigated whether a common variant of CNTNAP2 (rs7794745) is relevant for syntactic and semantic processing in the general population by using a visual sentence processing paradigm while recording ERPs in 49 healthy adults. While both AA homozygotes and T-carriers showed a standard N400 effect to semantic anomalies, the response to subject-verb agreement violations differed across genotype groups. T-carriers displayed an anterior negativity preceding the P600 effect, whereas for the AA group only a P600 effect was observed. These results provide another piece of evidence that the neuronal architecture of the human faculty of language is shaped differently by effects that are genetically determined.

References

[1]  Stromswold K (2001) The heritability of language: a review and metaanalysis of twin, adoption and linkage studies. Language 77: 647–723.
[2]  Ramus F, Fisher SE (2009) Genetics of Language. In: Gazzaniga MS, editor. The New Cognitive Neuroscience. 4th ed. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 855–871.
[3]  Bishop DV (2009) Genes, cognition, and communication: insights from neurodevelopmental disorders. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1156: 1–18.
[4]  Carlsson P, Mahlapuu M (2002) Forkhead transcription factors: key players in development and metabolism. Dev Biol 250: 1–23.
[5]  Fisher SE, Scharff C (2009) FOXP2 as a molecular window into speech and language. Trends Genet 25: 166–176.
[6]  Lai CSL, Gerrelli D, Monaco AP, Fisher SE, Copp AJ (2003) FOXP2 expression during brain development coincides with adult sites of pathology in a severe speech and language disorder. Brain 126: 2455–2462.
[7]  Watkins KE, Dronkers NF, Vargha-Khadem F (2002) Behavioural analysis of an inherited speech and language disorder: comparison with acquired aphasia. Brain 125: 452–464.
[8]  Lai CSL, Fisher SE, Hurst JA, Vargha-Khadem F, Monaco AP (2001) A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature 413: 519–523.
[9]  MacDermot KD, Bonora E, Sykes N, Coupe AM, Lai CSL, et al. (2005) Identification of FOXP2 truncation as a novel cause of developmental speech and language deficits. Am J Hum Genet 76: 1074–1080.
[10]  Shriberg LD, Ballard KJ, Tomblin JB, Duffy JR, Odell KH, et al. (2006) Speech, prosody, and voice characteristics of a mother and daughter with a 7;13 translocation affecting FOXP2. J Speech Lang Hear Res 49: 500–525.
[11]  Feuk L, Kalervo A, Lipsanen-Nyman M, Skaug J, Nakabayashi K, et al. (2006) Absence of a paternally inherited FOXP2 gene in developmental verbal dyspraxia. Am J Hum Genet 79: 965–972.
[12]  Liegeois F, Baldeweg T, Connelly A, Gadian DG, Mishkin M, et al. (2003) Language fMRI abnormalities associated with FOXP2 gene mutation. Nat Neurosci 6: 1230–1237.
[13]  Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian DG, Copp A, Mishkin M (2005) FOXP2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and language. Nat Rev Neurosci 6: 131–138.
[14]  Belton E, Salmond CH, Watkins KE, Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian DG (2003) Bilateral brain abnormalities associated with dominantly inherited verbal and orofacial dyspraxia. Hum Brain Mapp 18: 194–200.
[15]  Watkins KE, Gadian DG, Vargha-Khadem F (1999) Human genetics ‘99. Functional and structural brain abnormalities associated with a genetic disorder of speech and language. Am J Hum Genet 65: 1215–1221.
[16]  Watkins KE, Vargha-Khadem F, Ashburner J, Passingham RE, Connelly A, et al. (2002) MRI analysis of an inherited speech and language disorder: structural brain abnormalities. Brain 125: 465–478.
[17]  Fisher SE, Marcus GF (2006) The eloquent ape: genes, brains and the evolution of language. Nat Rev Genet 7: 9–20.
[18]  Scharff C, Petri J (2011) Evo-devo, deep homology and FoxP2: implications for the evolution of speech and language. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 366: 2124–2140.
[19]  Vernes SC, Newbury F, Abrahams BS, Winchester L, Nicod J, et al. (2008) A functional genetic link between distinct developmental language disorders. N Engl J Med 359: 2337–2345.
[20]  Nakabayashi K, Scherer SW (2001) The human contactin-associated protein-like 2 gene (CNTNAP2) spans over 2 Mb of DNA at chromosome 7q35. Genomics 73: 108–112.
[21]  Abrahams BS, Tentler D, Perederiy JV, Oldham MC, Coppola G, et al. (2007) Genome-wide analyses of human perisylvian cerebral cortical patterning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104: 17849–17854.
[22]  Zweier C, de Jong EK, Zweier M, Orrico A, Ousager LB, et al. (2009) CNTNAP2 and NRXN1 are mutated in autosomal-recessive Pitt-Hopkins-like mental retardation and determine the level of a common synaptic protein in Drosophila. Am J Hum Genet 85: 655–666.
[23]  Gregor A, Albrecht B, Bader I, Bijlsma EK, Ekici AB, et al. (2011) Expanding the clinical spectrum associated with defects in CNTNAP2 and NRXN1. BMC Med Genet 12: 106.
[24]  Strauss KA, Puffenberger EG, Huentelman MJ, Gottlieb S, Dobrin SE, et al. (2006) Recessive symptomatic focal epilepsy and mutant contactin-associated protein-like 2. N Engl J Med 354: 1370–1377.
[25]  Alarcon M, Abrahams BS, Stone JL, Duvall JA, Perederiy JV, et al. (2008) Linkage, association, and gene-expression analyses identify CNTNAP2 as an autism-susceptibility gene. Am J Hum Genet 82: 150–159.
[26]  Steer CD, Golding J, Bolton PF (2010) Traits contributing to the autistic spectrum. PLoS One 5: e12633.
[27]  Poot M, Beyer V, Schwaab I, Damatova N, Van’t Slot R, et al. (2010) Disruption of CNTNAP2 and additional structural genome changes in a boy with speech delay and autism spectrum disorder. Neurogenetics 11: 81–89.
[28]  Newbury DF, Paracchini S, Scerri TS, Winchester L, Addis L, et al. (2011) Investigation of dyslexia and SLI risk variants in reading- and language-impaired subjects. Behav Genet 41: 90–104.
[29]  Peter B, Raskind WH, Matsushita M, Lisowski M, Vu T, et al. (2011) Replication of CNTNAP2 association with nonword repetition and support for FOXP2 association with timed reading and motor activities in a dyslexia family sample. J Neurodev Disord 3: 39–49.
[30]  Whitehouse AJ, Bishop DV, Ang QW, Pennell CE, Fisher SE (2011) CNTNAP2 variants affect early language development in the general population. Genes Brain Behav 10: 451–456.
[31]  Whalley HC, O’Connell G, Sussmann JE, Peel A, Stanfield AC, et al. (2011) Genetic variation in CNTNAP2 alters brain function during linguistic processing in healthy individuals. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 156B: 941–948.
[32]  Snijders TM (2010) More than words. Neural and genetic dynamics of syntactic unification. Nijmegen, ISBN 978–90–9025391–6: Radboud University Nijmegen. DOI: http://hdl.handle.net/2066/77569. Radboud Repository website. Available http://repository.ubn.ru.nl/bitstream/20?66/77569/1/77569.pdf accessed 2012 September 23.
[33]  Folia V, Forkstam C, Ingvar M, Hagoort P, Petersson KM (2011) Implicit artificial syntax processing: genes, preference, and bounded recursion. Biolinguistics 5: 105–132.
[34]  Arking DE, Cutler DJ, Brune CW, Teslovich TM, West K, et al. (2008) A common genetic variant in the neurexin superfamily member CNTNAP2 increases familial risk of autism. Am J Hum Genet 82: 160–164.
[35]  Hagoort P, Brown C, Groothusen J (1993) The syntactic positive shift (Sps) as an ERP measure of syntactic processing. Lang Cogn Process 8: 439–483.
[36]  Kos M, Vosse T, Van den Brink D, Hagoort P (2010) About edible restaurants:conflicts between syntax and semantics as revealed by ERPs. Front Psychol 1: 222 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00222.
[37]  Hasting AS, Kotz SA (2008) Speeding up syntax: on the relative timing and automaticity of local phrase structure and morphosyntactic processing as reflected in event-related brain potentials. J Cogn Neurosci 20: 1207–1219.
[38]  Mancini S, Molinaro N, Rizzi L, Carreiras M (2011) A person is not a number: Discourse involvement in subject-verb agreement computation. Brain Res 1410: 64–76.
[39]  Hagoort P (2003) How the brain solves the binding problem for language: A neurocomputational model of syntactic processing. Neuroimage 20: S18–S29.
[40]  Hagoort P (2009) Reflections on the neurobiology of syntax. In: Bickerton D, Szathmáry E, editors. Biological foundations and origin of syntax. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.
[41]  Friederici AD (2002) Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6: 78–84.
[42]  Bornkessel-Schlesewsky I, Kretzschmar F, Tune S, Wang L, Genc S, et al. (2011) Think globally: Cross-linguistic variation in electrophysiological activity during sentence comprehension. Brain Lang 117: 133–152.
[43]  Van de Meerendonk N, Kolk HHJ, Chwilla DJ, Vissers CTWM (2009) Monitoring in language perception. Lang Linguist Compass 3: 1211–1224.
[44]  Molinaro N, Barber HA, Carreiras M (2011) Grammatical agreement processing in reading: ERP findings and future directions. Cortex 47: 908–930.
[45]  Osterhout L, Mobley LA (1995) Event-related brain potentials elicited by failure to agree. J Mem Lang 34: 739–773.
[46]  Friederici AD, Pfeifer E, Hahne A (1993) Event-related brain potentials during natural speech processing: effects of semantic, morphological and syntactic violations. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 1: 183–192.
[47]  Hagoort P, Hald L, Bastiaansen M, Petersson KM (2004) Integration of word meaning and world knowledge in language comprehension. Science 304: 438–441.
[48]  Van den Brink D, Brown CM, Hagoort P (2001) Electrophysiological evidence for early contextual influences during spoken-word recognition: N200 versus N400 effects. J Cogn Neurosci 13: 967–985.
[49]  Swaab TY, Brown C, Hagoort P (1997) Spoken sentence comprehension in aphasia: Event-related potential evidence for a lexical integration deficit. J Cogn Neurosci 9: 39–66.
[50]  Kutas M, Hillyard SA (1980) Reading senseless sentences - Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science 207: 203–205.
[51]  Kutas M, Hillyard SA (1984) Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Nature 307: 161–163.
[52]  Baggio G, Hagoort P (2011) The balance between memory and unification in semantics: towards a dynamic account of the N400. Lang Cogn Process 26: 1338–1367.
[53]  Kutas M, Federmeier KD (2011) Thirty Years and Counting: Finding Meaning in the N400 Component of the Event-Related Brain Potential (ERP). Annu Rev Psychol 62: 621–647.
[54]  Laszlo S, Federmeier KD (2011) The N400 as a snapshot of interactive processing: Evidence from regression analyses of orthographic neighbor and lexical associate effects. Psychophysiology 48: 176–186.
[55]  Baaijen RH, Piepenbrock R, Van Rijn H (1993) The CELEX lexical database (CD-ROM). Philadelphia: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.
[56]  Kolk HHJ, Chwilla DJ, Van Herten M, Oor PJW (2003) Structure and limited capacity in verbal working memory: A study with event-related potentials. Brain Lang 85: 1–36.
[57]  Van der Beek L, Bouma G, Malouf R, van Noord G, editors (2001) The alpino dependency treebank. Amsterdam: Rodopi. 8–22.
[58]  Van de Meerendonk N, Indefrey P, Chwilla DJ, Kolk HHJ (2011) Monitoring in language perception: Electrophysiological and hemodynamic responses to spelling violations. Neuroimage 54: 2350–2363.
[59]  Kuperberg GR, Sitnikova T, Lakshmanan BM (2008) Neuroanatomical distinctions within the semantic system during sentence comprehension: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 40: 367–388.
[60]  Pijnacker J, Geurts B, van Lambalgen M, Buitelaar J, Hagoort P (2010) Exceptions and anomalies: an ERP study on context sensitivity in autism. Neuropsychologia 48: 2940–2951.
[61]  Gold R, Faust M, Goldstein A (2010) Semantic integration during metaphor comprehension in Asperger syndrome. Brain Lang 113: 124–134.

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133