%0 Journal Article %T How many mailouts? Could attempts to increase the response rate in the Iraq war cohort study be counterproductive? %A A Rosemary Tate %A Margaret Jones %A Lisa Hull %A Nicola T Fear %A Roberto Rona %A Simon Wessely %A Matthew Hotopf %J BMC Medical Research Methodology %D 2007 %I BioMed Central %R 10.1186/1471-2288-7-51 %X Data for 17,162 UK military personnel were used to determine factors related to response and inverse probability weights were used to assess nonresponse bias. The percentages of inconsistent and missing answers to health questions from the 10,234 responders were used as measures of misclassification in a simulation of the 'true' relative risks that would have been observed if misclassification had not been present. Simulated and observed relative risks of multiple physical symptoms and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) were compared across response waves (number of contact attempts).Age, rank, gender, ethnic group, enlistment type (regular/reservist) and contact address (military or civilian), but not fitness, were significantly related to response. Weighting for nonresponse had little effect on the relative risks. Of the respondents, 88% had responded by wave 2. Missing answers (total 3%) increased significantly (p < 0.001) between waves 1 and 4 from 2.4% to 7.3%, and the percentage with discrepant answers (total 14%) increased from 12.8% to 16.3% (p = 0.007). However, the adjusted relative risks decreased only slightly from 1.24 to 1.22 for multiple physical symptoms and from 1.12 to 1.09 for PTSD, and showed a similar pattern to those simulated.Bias due to nonresponse appears to be small in this study, and increasing the response rates had little effect on the results. Although misclassification is difficult to assess, the results suggest that bias due to reporting errors could be greater than bias caused by nonresponse. Resources might be better spent on improving and validating the data, rather than on increasing the response rate.Poor response is a major source of concern in epidemiological surveys, and much effort is often spent on chasing up initial non-responders [1] with the implicit assumption that a higher response rate is associated with a more representative sample and hence lower bias. However, there is increasing evidence that this assumption may %U http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2288/7/51